Series shifts and mergers in the obstruent phonology of Tahltan
(Northern Athabaskan)
John
Alderete, Simon Fraser University
Amber Blenkiron, Simon Fraser University
Edōsdi (Judy Thompson), University of Victoria
A survey was
conducted to investigate the development of the Proto-Athabaskan obstruent
series, *ts/tš/tšr/k, into present day Tahltan. Results from
seven native speakers and quantitative analysis of a larger corpus establish tθ/ts/ts/tš as the standard obstruent system, alongside three alternate
systems that relate to independently motivated historical changes. These
findings support the long-held view that differences in the obstruent reflexes
do not reflect deep phonological differences among Northern Athabaskan languages,
but instead represent areal influences and patterns of individual variation in
a highly dynamic language network.
1. Introduction
A recurring theme in Athabaskan linguistics is that the task
of classifying Northern Athabaskan languages into historically meaningful
subgroups is fraught with a myriad of contact phenomena. Instead of traditional
tree-based subgroupings, contemporary research tends to analyze similarities
among languages as the result of waves of structural features across groups that
have had extended contact (Krauss
1964; Krauss 1973; Krauss & Golla 1981; Rice 2004). Attempts to posit taxonomic
subgroupings, as in Hoijer
(1963), lead to major divisions among
languages that are obviously closely related. The language complex of Tagish/Tahltan/Kaska is a case in point. Krauss and Golla
(1981) argue that these languages are nearly identical in lexicon and grammar,
but they have three rather different obstruent systems, defying analyses that
these differences reflect deep historical divergences.
We accept the fact of prolonged contact
in these languages, but would like to point out that, in some cases, the
factual basis motivating the wave model was based originally on limited
information. More recent research has shown that the obstruent system of the
standard variety of Tahltan is parallel to Kaska,
with both languages shifting Proto-Athabaskan *ts/tš/tšr/k to tθ/ts/ts/tš (Hardwick
1984; Nater 1989). This fact weakens the language
contact argument somewhat, because the overlap in grammar and lexicon
correlates with the same shifts.
The main goal of this report is
to investigate the series shifts and mergers in Tahltan and relate our findings
to Athabaskan historical phonology. In particular, we examine the speech of
seven native speakers with a questionnaire designed to elicit the development
of four obstruent series. In addition to documenting the standard variety, our
results reveal two new obstruent systems in active use. It turns out that all
of these systems, including a fourth system assumed in Krauss and Golla (1981), are either identical to those of neighboring
languages or can be derived from them through independently motivated
processes. Thus, while initially a cause for concern, Tahltan actually seems to
strengthen Krauss and Golla’s argument for the wave
model. It is not simply the case that three closely-related
languages have very different obstruent systems. The same language has four.
The rest of this article is
organized as follows. In section 2 we situate the language and describe some of
the dynamics of Tahltan speaking communities relevant to our analysis. Section
3 provides the historical perspective necessary for understanding structural
similarities and differences that exist among Northern Athabaskan languages. In
section 4, we give a quantitative analysis of the distribution of obstruents in a corpus of 455 stems, documenting the
structure of Tahltan with more rigor and some statistical facts relevant to the
development of obstruents. Section 5 presents the
results of an investigation of eight native speakers and analyzes the four
attested systems within Northern Athabaskan historical phonology. Section 6
gives some concluding remarks.
2. Language
background
Tahltan
is a Northern Athabaskan language of Northwestern British Columbia, Canada. It
is spoken by fewer than 50 speakers, mostly concentrated in the communities of Dease Lake, Iskut, and Telegraph
Creek. Using traditional sociolinguistic criteria, Krauss & Golla (1981) assume that Kaska
and Tagish are the most closely related languages. Historical ties with other
Athabaskan groups have also been documented for Sekani,
Tsetaut, Carrier, Slavey, and Witsuwit’en,
as well as with coastal Tlingit, Taku Tlingit, Gitksan/Nisga’a, Haida, Cree, and Coast Tsimshian (Thomas
McIlwraith, personal communication, and our consultant interviews). The identification
of distinct Tahltan dialects and varieties is not a simple matter because we
began research on the language while it was in a moribund state, and the
salient linguistic features observed in inter-speaker variation do not
correlate straightforwardly with known communities or immigration patterns.
However, the results of our survey in section 5 describe one of the most important
facets of this variation.
Some
facts about Tahltan ethnography and the lives of the native speaker consultants
provide additional context. All of the consultants were bi-lingual in English
and Tahltan, and many of them are also fluent in, or had significant contact
with, additional languages. The consultants were also raised in their
traditional territory prior to the opening up of northwestern British Columbia
with the construction of the Cassiar-Stewart Highway.
As a result, the elder consultants led very traditional lives, engaging in
subsistence hunting and fishing and regularly traversing long distances, often
on foot. The consultants also have rather diverse historical backgrounds, with
one being born in Shesley, a historically Kaska-speaking area, two others with ancestral ties to Bear
Lake Sekani people, and still others with historical
ties to Tlingit and Cree people. As has been remarked in other ethnographic
accounts of Tahltan society (McIlwraith 2012; Sheppard 1983), these communities are dynamic places where there is constant contact
with other Athabaskan speaking and non-Athabaskan speaking language groups.
We would
like to note that members of these communities sometimes refer to their
language with anglicized Tahltan, but
also sometimes as Tāłtān
[tɑːɬtɑːn], and the latter name is written as such,
using the Tahltan orthography, in web resources such as the First Peoples’
Cultural Council language map. We use the name Tahltan because it is more common in linguistic circles,
and employ the same practice with other Northern Athabaskan language
names. However, we refer our readers to the language maps of First Peoples’
Cultural Council (maps.fphlcc.ca) and First Voices (www.firstvoices.com) for
more information on how native groups may prefer to refer to their languages.
3. Shifts and mergers in Northern
Athabaskan
To
understand present-day Tahltan, we must first understand a set of sound changes
that have been proposed to account for Northern Athabaskan languages. The
inventory in (1) represents the reconstructed consonants of Proto-Athabaskan (Krauss 1964; Krauss & Golla 1981; Leer 1979; Rice
1994). The
system of contrasts in (1) is usually described in stems, which are canonically
monosyllabic and typically have CV(C) prosodic structure. The full range of
consonants was available stem-initially in Proto-Athabaskan (PA), but
stem-finally the three-way laryngeal contrast in stops was neutralized to a
two-way contrast between voiceless unaspirated stops and ejectives. The
retroflex-palatal series, reconstructed
originally in Krauss
(1964) as a fronted velar series with
a labial component, *k̯ʷ,
are reconstructed to explain the ts: tr opposition
in Alaskan languages like Minto and Ingalik. Laterals
ɬ and l are typically grouped with fricatives because they often pattern
with fricatives in voicing alternations.
(1)
Proto-Athabaskan consonants
Obstruents
|
dental
|
lateral
|
alveolar
|
palatal
|
retro-pal
|
velar
|
uvular
|
glottal
|
vls unasp
stop
|
d
|
dl
|
dz
|
dž
|
džr
|
g
|
G
|
ʔ
|
vls asp stop
|
t
|
tɬ
|
ts
|
tš
|
tšr
|
k
|
q
|
|
ejective
stop
|
t’
|
tɬ’
|
ts’
|
tš’
|
tš’r
|
l’
|
q’
|
|
vls fricative
|
|
ɬ
|
s
|
š
|
šr
|
x
|
χ
|
h
|
vd fricative
|
|
l
|
z
|
ž
|
žr
|
ɣ
|
ʀ
|
|
Sonorants
No descendent of Proto-Athabaskan retains the entire set of
consonants. The dental, lateral, and glottal series tend to be stable
historically. The other five place series, boxed in (1), have undergone several
shifts and mergers that are often used as features for classifying Northern
Athabaskan languages. These sound changes usually apply to all obstruents in the series, and not in piecemeal fashion. The
most important of these changes for our study is a chain shift dubbed the Great
Northern Series Shift, described below.
(2)
Great Northern Series Shift (=GNSS, after Leer 1996)
Stage
1. Fronting of the affricate series: alveolar obstruents
shift to interdentals and palatals shift to alveolar place, e.g., *ts > tθ, *tš
> ts.
Stage
2. Fronting of palatals and uvular onsets: velars (a.k.a. fronted velars)
become palatals and uvulars become velars in syllable
onsets, e.g., *k > tš, q > k / σ[__.
Retroflex palatals had merged with palatals in most
languages prior to the GNSS, so they are generally fronted as palatals in stage
1. A few Northern Athabaskan languages only underwent stage 1, e.g., the Alaskan
languages Lower Koyukon and Holikachuk,
but the majority underwent both stages. Northern Athabaskan languages can be
further cross-classified by mergers affecting front (alveolar and palatal) and
back (palatal and velar) place classes. These differences are shown below in
(3) with a sampling of related languages. The languages below are chosen
because of their potential contact with Tahltan, either through geographic
proximity or known ancestral ties. We will return to these relationships in
section 5. Kaska, for example, continues the five
place classes as four: tθ/ts/ts/tš/k, merging PA palatals
and retroflex palatals to ts and shifting all consonants forward via the GNSS. Sekani is likewise believed to have undergone both stages,
but the two front classes resulting from stage 1 of the GNSS shift merged into
a single alveolar place: *ts/tš/tšr/k/q > ts/ts/ts/tš/k. Witsuwit’en, on the
other hand, only underwent stage 1 of the GNSS, as shown by velar and uvular
reflexes, but it seems to share with Sekani and other
neighboring languages the merger of *tθ ts
> ts.
(3)
Structural features of Northern Athabaskan languages in contact with Tahltan
|
Tone
|
Obst]St
|
C’]St
|
*ts/tš>ts
|
*tš/tšr>tš
|
*tš/k>tš
|
*ts/tš/tšr/k/q
|
No. Tutchone
|
high
|
no
|
NA
|
no
|
yes
|
no
|
tθ ts ts tš
k
|
So. Tutchone
|
low
|
no
|
NA
|
no
|
yes
|
no
|
tθ ts ts tš
k
|
Tagish
|
low
|
no
|
NA
|
yes
|
yes
|
no
|
ts ts ts tsy
k
|
Tsetsaut
|
absent
|
no
|
NA
|
yes
|
no
|
no
|
ts ts pf tš k
|
Kaska
|
high
|
yes
|
no
|
no
|
yes
|
no
|
tθ ts ts tš
k
|
Tahltan 1
|
(low)
|
yes
|
no
|
no
|
yes
|
no
|
tθ ts ts tš
k
|
Tahltan
2
|
(low)
|
yes
|
no
|
no
|
yes
|
yes
|
ts tš tš tš
k
|
Sekani
|
low
|
yes
|
no
|
yes
|
yes
|
no
|
ts ts ts tš
k
|
HW
Beaver
|
low
|
yes
|
no
|
yes
|
yes
|
no
|
ts ts ts tš
k
|
D/B
Beaver
|
high
|
yes
|
no
|
yes
|
yes
|
no
|
tθ ts ts tš
k
|
Witsuwit’en
|
absent
|
yes
|
no
|
yes
|
yes
|
no
|
ts ts ts k q
|
Where does Tahltan fit into the
picture? Tahltan is similar to its neighbors to the north (Kaska)
and east (Beaver, Sekani) in its retention of stem
final obstruents. This is in contrast to Tahltan’s
neighbors to the north (Tutchone, Kutchin) and west (Tagish,
Tsetsaut) that neutralized stem-final obstruents to default coronal and velar stops. But like
Tagish and Southern Tutchone, and unlike Kaska,
Tahltan developed low-marked tone. See Krauss
and Golla (1981) for a more comprehensive
analysis of these features and important references, and the following works
for more detailed accounts of individual languages: Moore
(2002) on Kaska,
Hargus
(1985) on Sekani,
Randoja
(1989) on Beaver, and Hargus
(2007) on Witsuwit’en.
The development of affricates in
Tahltan is less clear. The data supporting the first pattern, Tahltan 2 in
Table 1 (Krauss
& Golla 1981; Story 1975), was collected by Kenneth Hale
and Geoff O’Grady in 1965 from a Tahltan elder, Pete Henyu.
Mr. Henyu was 79 at the time of the recording, so the
data he provided illustrates the language of an elder born 14 years before the
turn of the twentieth century. Since this work, two studies have collected data
that support a different analysis (Tahltan 1). Hardwick (1984) re-examines the
development of affricates with data from several elders from the Telegraph
Creek area and argues for a more conservative historical analysis *ts/tš/tšr/k/q > tθ/ts/ts/tš/k, retaining the distinction between *tš: k as ts: tš. Nater (1989) comes to a similar
conclusion, based on his extensive study of Tahltan spoken in Iskut.
The conservative analysis of
Hardwick and Nater has informed linguistic documentation work conducted in the
1980’s and 1990’s, including the children’s dictionary and the creation of an
orthography (Carter
& Council 1994; Leer 1985). The interdentals, which are
not posited in Story’s analysis, are used in the standard pronunciations of
many words. The contemporary Tahltan sound inventory given below with phonetic
symbols reflects this standard (see Carter (1994) and Alderete
and Blenkiron (2014) for the conversions of these
sounds to the Tahltan orthography).
(4)
Tahltan consonants
|
labial
|
dental
|
lateral
|
interdental
|
alveolar
|
palatal
|
velar
|
uvular
|
glottal
|
vd
|
b
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
vls
|
|
d
|
dl
|
dð
|
dz
|
dž
|
g
gʷ
|
|
ʔ
|
vls asp
|
|
t
|
tɬ
|
tθ
|
ts
|
ts
|
k
kʷ
|
(q)
|
|
ejective
|
|
t’
|
tɬ’
|
tθ’
|
ts’
|
ts’
|
k’
|
(q’)
|
|
vls fric
|
|
|
ɬ
|
θ
|
s
|
s
|
x
xʷ
|
|
h
|
vd fric
|
|
|
l
|
ð
|
z
|
z
|
ɣ
ɣʷ
|
|
|
nasal
|
m
|
|
|
|
n n̥
n’
|
|
|
|
|
sonorant
|
w
|
|
|
|
|
y
|
|
|
|
Following standard practice in Athabaskan, the
aspirated/unaspirated distinction is written t: d and k: g, though b is actually a voiced stop (Bob
1999). Also, uvulars
seem to be more characteristic of an older generation, as many speakers today
have merged them with the corresponding velars and some Tahltan uvulars may have come from Tlingit (Hardwick 1984; Nater
1989).
This background seems to cast
some doubt on Story’s original analysis *ts/tš/tšr/k > ts/tš/tš/tš. Furthermore, the first author was
given a copy of the Pete Henyu tapes by Ken Hale and he has played them to several Tahltan elders. Many
elders have commented on the unusual nature of Mr. Henyu’s
speech. It is described as having some unfamiliar characteristics, including
its rhythm, use of particles, and double subject marking. If it is true that
this pattern is not representative of a larger group, then this finding would
weaken somewhat the claim that the different mergers found in the
Tagish/Tahltan/Kaska complex are superficial in
nature, and do not reflect deep structural differences among these languages.
The developments *ts/tš/tšr/k/q > tθ/ts/ts/tš/k into Tahltan would seem to be just one of a number
of features it has in common with Kaska and other
languages further east, including Sekani and Beaver
dialects, a point emphasized in Hardwick (1984). On the other hand, if we find
further evidence for the Henyu system within Tahltan,
this would provide even stronger evidence for the
superficial nature of the series mergers and shifts. It would show that the
variation attested in the larger language complex exists at the subdialectal level as well. It is against this background
that we investigate the variation and developments in the obstruent systems in
more detail.
4. Stem phonotactics
Many of the structural features used to relate Tahltan to
its neighbours, like the development of affricates, have
been based on somewhat limited data. In what follows, we situate the obstruent
phonology within a quantitative analysis of stem phonotactics. Such an analysis
is warranted in its own right, because of the increasing importance of
probabilistic effects in phonology (e.g., Pierrehumbert
(2003)). An account of the frequencies
of phonological segments also provides new insight into some of the
developments discussed in section 5.
To describe stem phonotactics,
we constructed a stem list from a larger wordlist of 1,038 words created from
our own fieldwork, three other primary linguistic descriptions of the language
((Hardwick
1984; Nater 1989; Nater 2006), and the children’s dictionary (Carter
& Council 1994). We excluded grammatical
morphemes and function words, and also adjectives and adverbs, which are often
morphologically complex. This resulted in a list of 455 stems consisting of
nouns, verbs, and postpositions. Stems in Athabaskan languages are usually
monosyllabic, so polysyllabic words had to be examined for morphological
complexity, including looking for known prefixes and the stem increment –e. For many disyllabic words, removing
these morphemes resulted in a CVC stem, which was included in the stem list as
such.
The tables in (5) give the
frequencies of stems sorted by the number of syllables and their CV structure.
These frequencies show that there are several disyllabic stems, e.g., keneθ ‘raft’, but
the overwhelming majority of stems are monosyllabic (92.7%). As for stem shape,
most stems end in a consonant (81.5%).
(5)
|
a.
Stem size frequencies
|
|
b.
Stem shape frequencies
|
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
|
CV
|
CVV
|
CVC
|
CVVC
|
CVCC
|
|
422
|
29
|
4
|
|
11
|
67
|
220
|
121
|
3
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
These facts support the monosyllabic CVC structure as the
canonical stem shape. They also confirm the (near total) lack of clusters
stem-finally, an important areal feature characteristic of neighboring Northern
Athabaskan languages like Kaska and Sekani.
Moving to the distribution of
consonants, the type frequencies of consonants are given below, sorted by stem-initial
and final position. Given the rarity of certain sounds, and transcription
inconsistency across sources, we had to collapse a few sounds into a single
category. For example, there are only three instances of rounded velars, so
these were merged with their corresponding unrounded velars. Three instances of
χ were likewise grouped with the
voiceless velar fricative. The collapsing of these categories is motivated
empirically because rounded velars were either lost or nearly so, and uvulars are replaced by corresponding velars by most
speakers (Nater
1989). Finally, voiceless and
glottalized nasals were collapsed with the alveolar nasal, but again, there
were only a handful of these. The true counts can be recovered with the above
information, but we wish to have a good count of the place of articulation
classes, which does not change with these adjustments.
(6) Consonant frequencies
a. Labial
|
|
b. Dental
|
|
b
|
m
|
w
|
|
|
d
|
t
|
t’
|
initial
|
12
|
5
|
0
|
|
initial
|
51
|
27
|
23
|
final
|
0
|
4
|
2
|
|
final
|
16
|
33
|
0
|
total
|
12
|
9
|
2
|
|
total
|
67
|
60
|
23
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
c.
Interdental
|
|
d.
Lateral
|
|
dð
|
tθ
|
tθ'
|
ð
|
θ
|
|
|
dl
|
tɬ
|
tɬ’
|
l
|
ɬ
|
initial
|
1
|
11
|
13
|
16
|
8
|
|
initial
|
7
|
4
|
15
|
18
|
9
|
final
|
1
|
6
|
0
|
1
|
23
|
|
final
|
3
|
4
|
0
|
7
|
44
|
total
|
2
|
17
|
13
|
17
|
31
|
|
total
|
10
|
8
|
15
|
25
|
53
|
e.
Alveolar
|
|
f.
Palatal
|
|
dz
|
ts
|
ts’
|
z
|
s
|
n
|
|
|
dž
|
tš
|
tš’
|
ž
|
š
|
y
|
initial
|
7
|
12
|
10
|
2
|
8
|
7
|
|
initial
|
6
|
23
|
5
|
0
|
0
|
16
|
final
|
3
|
6
|
0
|
5
|
24
|
56
|
|
final
|
3
|
3
|
0
|
1
|
8
|
0
|
total
|
10
|
18
|
10
|
7
|
32
|
63
|
|
total
|
9
|
26
|
5
|
1
|
8
|
16
|
g.
Velar
|
|
h.
Glottal
|
|
g
|
k
|
k'
|
x
|
ɣ
|
|
|
ʔ
|
h
|
initial
|
21
|
15
|
23
|
13
|
14
|
|
initial
|
21
|
0
|
final
|
2
|
7
|
0
|
4
|
1
|
|
final
|
38
|
41
|
total
|
23
|
22
|
23
|
17
|
15
|
|
total
|
59
|
41
|
For this data,
it is clear that certain sounds have a much higher frequency than others. We
sort consonants arbitrarily into bins based on multiples of 20 below, to give a
rough sense of the relative frequencies.
(7)
Frequency classes (in 455 stems)
High
(n > 40)
|
Medium
(20 < n < 39)
|
Low (3 < n
< 19)
|
Very
rare (n £ 2)
|
t d ł n ʔ h
|
θ t’ l s tš
g k k’
|
b m tθ
tθ’ ð dz ts ts’
dl tł tł’ z dž tš’
š y x ɣ
|
w dð
ž
|
These counts must be qualified
by restrictions on position. h has a
rather high frequency, but it only occurs in stem-final position. On the other
hand, š and ž never occur stem-initially. These sounds, especially ž, are questionable as phonemes of the
language because they were lost stem-initially (see Hardwick (1984)), and may be predictable from the regular rule of consonant
harmony (Hardwick
1984; Shaw 1991). There are also sounds that
never occur stem-finally: b, y, and ejectives as a class. These
frequencies show that the two-way distinction between plain and ejective stops
in Proto-Athabaskan stem-finals has been lost in Tahltan, a fact that is also consistent
with stem-finals in many of Tahltan’s neighbors to the north and east (see (3)).
The absence of stem-final b is not a
surprise, however, because it is generally the reflex of stem-initial *w (Krauss
& Leer 1981). We also note that velars as a
class have medium frequency, but they are clearly preferred stem-initially.
The counts shown below of the anticipated reflexes of PA obstruents give a sense of the robustness of the different
affricate classes. Most of the affricates and fricatives have medium to low
frequency, with the phoneme tš emerging as the clear leader. Four cases have very low
counts: dð,
which is rare both stem-initially and finally, z, also rare in general, and the palatal fricatives, which never
give an appearance stem-initially.
(8) Stem-initial affricates
*ts
|
*tš
|
*k
|
tθ
|
11
|
ts
|
12
|
tš
|
23
|
tθ’
|
13
|
ts’
|
10
|
tš’
|
5
|
dð
|
1
|
dz
|
7
|
dž
|
6
|
θ
|
8
|
s
|
8
|
š
|
0
|
ð
|
16
|
z
|
2
|
ž
|
0
|
Finally, the co-occurrence table below documents the interaction
of the place classes in stems (excluding glottals).
In this table, rows and columns indicate stem-initials and stem-finals,
respectively.
(9)
Co-occurrence of consonants
|
|
dent
|
lat
|
int-dent
|
alv
|
pal
|
vel
|
dental
|
d t t’
|
5
|
14
|
8
|
10
|
3
|
2
|
lateral
|
dl tɬ tɬ’ l
ɬ
|
9
|
5
|
1
|
2
|
0
|
4
|
interdental
|
dð tθ tθ’ ð θ
|
11
|
11
|
7
|
0
|
0
|
1
|
alveolar
|
dz ts ts’ z s n
|
4
|
2
|
0
|
8
|
0
|
2
|
palatal
|
dž tš tš’ ž š j
|
4
|
7
|
0
|
0
|
9
|
1
|
velar
|
g k k’ x ɣ
|
11
|
11
|
11
|
13
|
1
|
5
|
While most series freely combine with one another, the
coronal place classes referred to by the coronal harmony rule (Hardwick
1984; Shaw 1991), i.e., interdental, alveolar,
and palatal, do not freely co-occur. As shown in the boxed region, same-place
coronal categories are not restricted, but different-place combinations are
categorically avoided, which is clearly significant. The different-place
combinations are of course just those combinations that would be ruled out as
disharmonic roots. The lateral and dental series are not restricted in this
way, consistent with their transparent nature. These restrictions are expected
synchronically if coronal harmony is active in roots, but they are also
predicted historically, because Proto-Athabaskan stems have been shown to avoid
certain different-place combinations, including *ts and *tš (Krauss
1964).
5. Development of affricates
How did the Proto-Athabaskan affricate series develop into
present-day Tahltan, and what is the range of variation among speakers? To
answer this question, we constructed a 46-word questionnaire designed to elicit
the four principal place series (alveolar, palatal, palatal-retroflex, and velar)
in stem-initial obstruents. The questionnaire was
based on the PA reconstructions given in the appendix and shown in the table
headers below. The data were collected by the first author in 1999 and 2000
with seven fluent speakers. In each interview, he attempted to gain an initial
purchase of the data, checking for non-cognate forms and pronunciation detail.
Once the facts were reasonably clear, a second run through the questionnaire
was recorded with a tie-clip lavaliere microphone. Four of the native speakers
had either been raised in Telegraph Creek or had strong associations with this
community. The remaining three speakers were from Iskut.
The age of the native speakers ranged from 59 to 83, so these speakers were born
between 1917 and 1941. To confirm the transcriptions, each interview was
checked by either the second or third author.
Of our seven consultants, two pairs were couples and they
wished to be present at their spouse’s interview. Interestingly, both couples
demonstrated different obstruent patterns, even when presented with the rather
salient differences by their spouse. These differences were noted on more than
one occasion by the consultants, demonstrating that they were consciously aware
of at least some of the differences.
The next three tables illustrate
the data collected in characteristic speaker profiles. As expected from Hardwick
(1984) and Nater
(1989), the predominant pattern was a
merger of the two palatal series and a wholesale shift forward in the mouth.
This is the standard obstruent system used by language practitioners and
linguists. This pattern is illustrated below in (10) with the speech of an Iskut elder. He was born in Telegraph Creek on the reserve
across from Dry Town, but immigrated to Iskut later and attended school there for ten years. This
elder had historical ties with Bear Lake Sekani people
and therefore may have been influenced by speakers of Sekani.
However, the same conservative pattern of retaining a three-way contrast is
also found in three other speakers, two of whom have rather different backgrounds,
including attending school in Telegraph Creek and having significant contact
with Tlingit.
(10)
Speaker profile I: the tθ/ts/ts/tš standard
(representative of four speakers)
|
*ts > tθ
|
*tš > ts
|
*tšr > ts
|
*k > tš
|
vls
asp
|
tθeː ‘stone’
-tθiʔ ‘head’
tθen’ ‘meat’
detθoɪ ‘yellow’
|
tsaʔ ‘beaver’
-tsiye ‘grandfather’
detsiːts, detsiːdzi ‘red’
|
tsàːʔ ‘excrement’
tsets ‘firewood’
-tsex ‘cry’
|
-latšɪne ‘wrist’
-ketšɪne ‘ankle’
tšaː ‘rain’
-laštšoː ‘thumb’
|
vls
eject
|
tθ’aʔ ‘plate,
dish’
tθ’èː ‘thread’
tθ’enh ‘bone’
tθ’aːtl ‘diaper’
|
ts’ah ‘hat’
ts’ede ‘blanket’
ts’uː ‘spruce’
-ts’iʔe ‘guts’
|
nats’ih ‘wind blows’
-ts’əse ‘kidney’
|
tš’ohe ‘porcupine
quills’
tšɪde ‘veins,
gristle’
|
unasp
|
dðeɬ ‘mountain
(over-hanging bluff)’
|
dzeh ɬen ‘pitch’
-dzeke ‘inner ear’
|
dzeneθ ‘day’
-dzeːʔ ‘heart’
-dzaːke ‘shin’
|
nedžɪt ‘he’s
scared’
tšošk’aʔe ‘Canadian
Jay’
džaːni ‘here’
|
fric
|
-ðet ‘liver’
-ðàːt ‘mouth’
θaː ‘sand’
|
sɪni ‘I/me’
|
saː ‘sun’
sek ‘saliva’
|
xɪn ‘song’
xoh ‘brown bear’
xɪθ ‘hill,
knoll’
sʌs ‘black bear’
xʌs ‘scar’
|
While most of the place/manner classes shifted forward, the
velar fricatives did not; see e.g., ‘song’ and ‘brown bear’. Our reconstructed
velar fricatives are ‘front velars’ and not uvulars (see appendix), because uvulars
would not be expected to shift to palatals. While correspondences with ‘black
bear’ are generally irregular in Athabaskan (Huld
1983), the observed form, sʌs, is
interesting because if the stem-initial had shifted as expected, i.e., *xəš > šʌs, it would have produced a disharmonic root,
contrary to the regular rules of coronal harmony (Hardwick
1984; Shaw 1991). As we saw in section 4,
Tahltan does not have any stems beginning with palatal fricatives, so it must
be that *š shifted forward, e.g.,
‘sun’, ‘saliva’, but the velar fricatives did not, leaving this slot empty.
There is a subtle variation on
this pattern that relates to the next speaker profile below. The speech of a
Telegraph Creek elder is broadly similar to profile I, but
retains the PA palatals in stem-initial aspirated stops. Thus, instead of the
alveolar affricates shown above in (10), this speaker says tšaʔ ‘beaver’, -tšiye
‘grandfather’, tših
‘red ochre’, and tšetš
‘firewood’, and varies between -tsex and -tšex for ‘cry’, cf. tsàːʔ ‘excrement’. It seems that
in this case the shift of the palatals to alveolars only occurred in
unaspirated stops, ejectives, and fricatives.
This piecemeal pattern is of
interest because another speaker from Telegraph Creek has the same pattern with
all the palatal stops, with a few exceptions. This elder’s speech is
illustrated below in (11). She was 83 at the time of the interview, the oldest
of our consultants, and was born in Shesley, which is
a Kaska-speaking area of British Columbia. However,
she had strong associations with Tahltan people from Telegraph Creek. This
elder has also had significant contact with both Tlingit and Haida.
(11)
Speaker profile II: tθ/tš/tš/tš
|
*ts > tθ
|
*tš > tš
|
*tšr > tš
|
*k > tš
|
vls
asp
|
tθeː ‘stone’
-tθiʔ ‘head’
tθen’ ‘meat’
detθoɪ ‘yellow’
|
tšaʔ ‘beaver’
-tšiye ‘grandfather’
|
tšàːʔ ‘excrement’
tšetš ‘firewood’
eš-tšaɪ ‘I cry’
|
-latšine ‘wrist’
-ketšine ‘ankle’
tšaː ‘rain’
-laštšoː ‘thumb’
|
vls
eject
|
tθ’aʔ ‘plate,
dish’
tθ’èː ‘thread’
tθ’enh ‘bone’
|
tš’ede ‘blanket’
tš’uː ‘spruce’
-tš’iʔe ‘guts’
cf. ts’ah ‘hat’
|
cf. -ts’ese ‘kidney’
|
tš’ohe ‘porcupine
quills’
tšide ‘veins,
gristle’
|
unasp
|
dðeɬ ‘mountain’
|
-džešbʌtle ‘ear’
džèːtl’en ‘pitch’
|
-džeː ‘heart’
cf. -dzodze ‘shin’
|
nedžɪt ‘he’s
scared’
tšoːsk’aʔe ‘Canadian
Jay’
džaːn ‘here’
|
fric
|
-ðet ‘liver’
-ðàːt ‘mouth, throat’
θaː ‘sand’
|
sini ‘I/me’
|
saː ‘sun’
|
(data unavailable)
|
We list ‘hat’, ‘kidney’, and ‘shin’ as exceptions here (they
shifted instead of retaining the ancient palatal), but we actually find them
difficult to classify. They might best be described as intermediate between ts and tš. We also note
that this elder has particularly clear examples of low-marked tone, e.g.,
‘thread’ and ‘mouth’, as well as a reluctance for lax ɪ in words like ‘wrist’ and ‘I/me’, which is found in most other
speakers.
The third pattern of development
is similar to profile I, except the *ts series does not shift forward to interdentals. Thus,
this speaker lacks interdentals altogether and simply opposes ts: tš. This elder was 73 at the time of
the interview and was married to a speaker with an obstruent system similar to
profile I. She was multi-lingual, with fluent or near fluent command of
English, French, Tahltan, as well as Cree (her mother was a Cree native
speaker) and Sekani (her father spoke Sekani). It is not known at what point she learned Tahltan,
but her fluency was confirmed in the interview.
(12)
Speaker profile III: ts/ts/ts/tš
|
*ts > ts
|
*tš > ts
|
*tšr > ts
|
*k > tš
|
vls
asp
|
tseː ‘stone’
-tsiʔ ‘head’
tsen’ ‘meat’
detsoɪ ‘yellow’
|
tsaʔ ‘beaver’
-tsiye ‘grandfather’
tsih ‘red ochre’
|
tsàːʔ ‘excrement’
tsets ‘firewood’
-tsex ‘I cry’
|
-latšɪne ‘wrist’
-ketšɪne ‘ankle’
tšaː ‘rain’
-laštšoː ‘thumb’
|
vls
eject
|
ts’aʔ ‘plate,
dish’
ts’èh ‘thread’
ts’enh ‘bone’
ts’aːtl ‘diaper’
|
ts’ah ‘hat’
ts’ede ‘blanket’
ts’uː ‘spruce’
-ts’iʔe ‘guts’
|
nats’ih ‘wind blows’
-ts’ese ‘kidney’
|
|
unasp
|
eya dzeɬe ‘high mountain’
|
dzehe ‘pitch’
-dzeke ‘inner ear’
|
dzeneθ ‘day’
-tseʔ ‘heart’
-dzada ‘shin’
|
nedžɪt ‘he’s
scared’
džaːn ‘here’
|
fric
|
-zet ‘liver’
-zat ‘mouth
saː ‘sand’
|
sɪni ‘I/me’
|
saː ‘sun’
sek ‘saliva’
|
xɪn ‘song’
xɪs ‘hill,
knoll’
sʌs ‘black bear’
xʌs ‘scar’
|
The table below lists the
results of our survey with profiles I-III, and compares
them with the speech of Pete Henyu (IV), who would
have been 114 in year 2000 (approximate date of interviews), or two generations
older than most of our consultants. The three patterns differ in the number of
resulting place contrasts (three vs. two) and the extent of the shifts. It is
not really possible to collapse the profiles by community, as profile I has
representatives, even with this small sample, from both communities.
(13)
Individual differences in shifts (I=Iskut,
TC=Telegraph Creek)
profile
|
community
|
age
at 2M
|
*ts
|
*tš
|
*tšr
|
*k
|
I
|
I
|
70
|
tθ
|
ts
|
ts
|
tš
|
I
|
I
|
78
|
tθ
|
ts
|
ts
|
tš
|
I
|
TC
|
59
|
tθ
|
ts
|
ts
|
tš
|
I
|
TC
|
61
|
tθ
|
ts
|
ts
|
tš
|
I-II
|
TC
|
72
|
tθ
|
tš/ts
|
tš/ts
|
tš
|
II
|
TC
|
83
|
tθ
|
tš
|
tš
|
tš
|
III
|
I
|
73
|
ts
|
ts
|
ts
|
tš
|
IV
|
TC
|
(114)
|
ts
|
tš
|
tš
|
tš
|
To
summarize, in addition to the conservative obstruent system in profile I, and IV
known from Pete Henyu’s speech, we have uncovered two
additional systems: one that resembles Tahltan’s neighbors to the south and
east with a merger in the front place classes (profile III), and one that
appears to merge ts
and tš from
the conservative system (profile II).
How do
these distinct systems fit within the historical perspective established in
section 3? In Figure 1, we visualize the Tahltan varieties based on accepted developments
in Northern Athabaskan languages (see section 3), showing how these variants
either relate to neighboring languages or can be derived from one of them. In
particular, we show developments from Proto-Athabaskan through both stages of
the Great Northern Series Shift and languages that appear to have undergone two
additional changes: Front Merger, as in Sekani, and
Back Merger, which seems to be attested in Tahltan only (see Krauss & Golla 1981, Figure 1a, for a similar breakdown and partial
overlap of component mergers and shifts). This chart is not intended as a
tree-based classification showing the accumulation of sound changes in unified
groups, but rather as a visual aid illustrating how the Tahltan varieties
relate to neighboring languages. Thus, we make no claims about sub-groupings
from the relationships depicted below. Indeed, there may be sideways arrows
that account for some of the shared mergers that are not depicted here.
Figure 1. Developments in obstruents
observed in Tahltan varieties and its neighbors
From
this visualization, we can see clearly how Tahltan varieties relate to other
Northern Athabaskan languages through their shared historical phonology. The
standard variety (I) is conservative, and so relates to Kaska,
Tutchone, and Doig/Blueberry Beaver. That is, Tahltan I underwent both stages
of the GNSS and no subsequent mergers, thus retaining the four-way place contrast.
Tahltan III is the same as Sekani and other neighbors
to the south and east. This pattern is likely the result of an influence from Sekani, as the sole speaker of this variety in our survey
is also fluent in Sekani, which was the language of
her father.
Patterns
IV and II are less straightforward. While profile IV is unique in Athabaskan (Krauss & Golla 1981), it can be accounted if we assume that it only underwent stage 2 of the
GNSS. That is, while most languages underwent both stages, and some languages
underwent just stage 1 of GNSS, perhaps in this speaker, the GNSS was not a
pull chain but simply a shift of velars and uvulars.
The only logical alternative is to assume that IV involved all stages of the
GNSS, plus Front Merger and Back Merger, but Occam’s Razor clearly favors the
first solution. Either way, however, it is clear that pattern IV relates to
sound changes that must have occurred in Northern Athabaskan.
Finally,
II is of interest because it presents two possibilities. It could be derived
from Tahltan IV, with a further shift of *ts > tθ. In other words, it could have
initially resisted stage 1 of the GNSS, underwent stage 2 with Tahltan IV, followed
by a shift of just alveolars (cf. stage 1). The second possibility, shown in
Figure 1, is for II to have developed from conservative Tahltan I via Back
Merger. While the existence of IV supports the first approach, two facts seem
to point to the second solution. First, at least one consultant vacillates
between pattern I and II, suggesting a change in progress from I to II, perhaps
frozen in the speech of this native speaker because of the general moribund
state of the language. Second, Back Merger is supported by the quantitative
analysis given in section 4. The phoneme tš has a strong statistical
advantage over ts (8), which can explain at least *ts tš > tš as a well-known
type of frequency effect (Blevins 2004; Greenberg 1966/2005). In pattern II, the merger of ts with high-frequency tš sound could
have led the larger stop series, bringing about change to the rest of the
alveolars as a class. The fact that the native speaker with I/II behavior shows
variation precisely in the voiceless aspirated stops, the statistical leader,
supports this analysis. The pattern in profile II, therefore, is not due to
contact, because the change seems to be an internal change based on frequency.
However, the Tahltan II profile does depend on the foundational change of the
GNSS, because it serves as the input to this pattern.
6. Concluding remarks
This article has investigated the development of affricates
in eight native speakers of Tahltan and found four distinct obstruent systems.
In the standard system (I), Proto-Athabaskan obstruents
shifted forward in accordance with both stages of the Great Northern Series
Shift, tθ/ts/tš/k, resembling its neighbors to
the north (Tutchone, Kaska) and east (Doig/Blueberry
Beaver). In another system (III), ts/ts/tš/k, the two front place classes merged
to ts, as
also observed in neighboring groups to the south and east (Half River Beaver, Sekani, Tsetsaut) and north
(Tagish). A third system (II) appears to be a development from I where ts has merged
with tš as
a frequency effect. Finally, IV, while unique in Athabaskan, seems to relate to
the rest of the systems by undergoing only stage 2 of the GNSS instead of both
stages. Thus, only half of the individual patterns documented here are identical
in structure, a fact that underscores the rather dynamic nature of the network
of Athabaskan-speaking language groups in this area.
The results also emphasize the
importance of individual-level variation and the need to engage with this kind
of variation in situating a Northern Athabaskan language with its neighbors. In
a sense, both Story’s original characterization of Tahltan, and Hardwick and Nater’s
analysis, are incomplete because they analyze only one facet of this variation.
Indeed, given the extent of the interaction with neighboring language groups,
we would not be surprised if an additional eight speakers uncovered new
obstruent systems. These details, and their ties to neighboring groups, are
part of the larger picture that characterizes this dynamic language network.
These results also provide some modest
practical suggestions for linguistic documentation and language. In particular,
they support the existence of a standard obstruent system, similar to that of Kaska. They also give one the tools to analyze three
alternate obstruent systems, II, III, IV, that can be recorded in dictionaries
as alternate pronunciations of standard forms, following the templates shown in
(13). As for teaching, identifying both a standard and alternate obstruent
systems may also help language learners grapple with the extensive inter- and
intra-speaker variation that exists. We would like to emphasize, however, that
adoption of a standard system is not a prescriptive statement that this system
is the official version, or “authentic Tahltan”, and that other systems are
marginal or unauthentic in any way. Every language embraces
variation, and so all of the profiles we document here represent valid pronunciations
of Tahltan words.
Finally, this article raises
some issues for further research. It is generally assumed that the mergers and
shifts discussed in this article applied to the entire series and not in
piecemeal fashion. We have discussed one case where this did not seem to be the
case, where voiceless aspirated stops resisted shifting (profile I/II), which
we assume is a frequency effect and perhaps a frozen partial change. The other
glaring example involves the avoidance of velar fricatives to shift to
palatals, as illustrated in both profiles I and III. Future work may look to
internal or external evidence to explain this dis-unity in an otherwise uniform
set of consonant shifts.
Appendix
The four tables below consolidate the information about the
forms in the affricates questionnaire, essentially
expanding on each of the four columns from the charts in section 3. For each
word, we give the historically reconstructed Proto-Athabaskan form (based on
commonly cited references, including Leer
(1979), Leer
(1987), and Krauss
(1979/2005)), the phonetic forms
illustrating the observed variation (the first phonetic form is the standard),
and finally the orthographic form of the standard form; see Carter (1994), Leer
(1985), and Alderete
and Blenkiron (2014) for details of the Tahltan
spelling system. Conventions: VT = vowel constriction.
(A)
Proto-Athabaskan alveolar series *ts
|
Reconstructed
form
|
Phonetic
forms
|
Orthographic
form
|
a.
vls asp
|
*tseː ‘stone’
|
tθeː, tseː
|
ts̱ē
|
|
*-tsỉʔ
‘head’
|
-tθiʔ, -tsiʔ
|
ts̱i’
|
|
*tsə̓n’
‘meat’
|
tθen’, tsen’
|
ts̱en’
|
|
*lə+tsʊɣ ‘yellow’
|
detθoɪ, detsoɪ,
|
dets̱oy
|
b.
vls eject
|
*ts’a̓ːk’ ‘dish’
|
tθ’aʔ, ts’aʔ
|
ts̱’a’
|
|
*ts’e̓ːχ ‘thread’
|
tθ’èː, ts’èh
|
ts̱’ē
|
|
*ts’ən ‘bone’
|
tθ’enh, ts’enh
|
ts̱’enh
|
|
*ts’aːtl’ ‘diaper’
|
tθ’aːtl, ts’aːtl
|
ts̱’ātl
|
c.
unasp
|
*dzəɬ ‘mountain’
|
dðeɬ, eya dzeɬe ‘high mountain’
|
dẕeł
|
d.
fricative
|
*sə̓t’
‘liver’
|
-ðet, -zet
|
ẕet
|
|
*-sa̓ːd ‘mouth’
|
-ðàːt, -zat
|
ẕāt
|
|
*saːxy ‘sand’
|
θaː, saː
|
s̱ā
|
(B)
Proto-Athabaskan palatal series *tš
a.
vls asp
|
*tšaʔ ‘beaver’
|
tsaʔ, tšaʔ
|
tsa’
|
|
*-tšə̓yə
‘grandfather’
|
-tsiye, tšiye
|
tsiye
|
|
*tšixy ‘(red) ochre’
|
tsiːts, tsiːdzi, tsih
|
tsīts
|
b.
vls eject
|
*tš’əχd ‘hat’
|
ts’ah
|
ts’ah
|
|
*tš’ədəʔ ‘blanket’
|
ts’ede, tš’ede
|
ts’ede
|
|
*tš’əwə ‘spruce’
|
ts’uː, tš’uː
|
ts’ū
|
|
*-tš’iːk’ ‘guts’
|
-ts’iʔe, -tš’iʔe
|
ts’i’e
|
c.
unasp
|
*dže̓ːχ ‘pitch’
|
dzehɬen, džèːtl’en, dzehe
|
dzehłen
|
|
*-džəɣəʔ ‘inner ear’
|
-dzeke, -džešbʌtle
‘ear’
|
dzeke
|
d.
fricative
|
*šiː ‘I/me’
|
sɪni, sini
|
sini
|
(C)
Proto-Athabaskan retroflex-palatal series *tšr
a.
vls asp
|
*tšra̓ːn’ ‘excrement’
|
tsàːʔ, tšàːʔ
|
tsā’
|
|
*tšrətšr ‘firewood’
|
tsets, tšetš
|
tsets
|
|
*Æ+tšrəχ
‘cry’
|
-tsex, -tšaɪ
|
tsex
|
b.
vls eject
|
*tšr’əy/tšr’iː
‘wind/blow’
|
nats’ih
|
nats’ih
|
|
*-tšr’ətšr’ ‘kidney’
|
-ts’əse, -ts’ese
|
ts’ese
|
c.
unasp
|
*džreːn ‘day’
|
dzeneθ
|
dzenes̱
|
|
*-džreːyəʔ ‘heart’
|
-dzeːʔ, -džeː, -tseʔ
|
dzē’
|
|
*-džraːdəʔ ‘shin’
|
-dzaːke, -dzodze, -dzada
|
dzāke
|
d.
fricative
|
*šraː ‘sun’
|
saː
|
sā
|
|
*šre̓ːχ ‘saliva’
|
sek
|
sek
|
(D)
Proto-Athabaskan velars *k
a. vls asp
|
*la+kən ‘wrist’
|
-latšɪne, -latšine
|
lachine
|
|
*ke+kən ‘ankle’
|
-ketšɪne, -ketšine
|
kechine
|
|
*kaːn ‘rain’
|
tšaː
|
chā
|
|
*la+kʊ̓ts’
‘thumb’
|
-laštšoː
|
lashchō
|
b.
vls eject
|
*k’əx ‘porcupine quills’
|
tš’ohe
|
ch’ohe
|
|
*k’ủːts’
‘veins, gristle’
|
tšɪde, tšide
|
chide
|
c.
unasp
|
*n+lə+guTːd ‘be afraid’
|
nedžɪt
|
nejit
|
|
*gỉzə
‘Canadian Jay’
|
tšošk’aʔe,
|
choshk’a’e
|
|
*ga ‘here’
|
džaːni, džaːn
|
jāni
|
d.
fricative
|
*xən ‘(shaman’s) song’
|
xɪn
|
khin
|
|
*x…ts’ ‘brown bear’
|
xoh
|
khoh
|
|
*xə̓ts’
‘hill, knoll’
|
xɪθ, xɪs
|
khis̱
|
|
*xa’t’ ‘scar’
|
xʌs
|
khas
|
|
*xəš ‘black bear’
|
sʌs
|
sas
|
N.b.: *xa’t’ ‘scar’ has constriction.
7. References
Alderete,
John & Amber Blenkiron. 2014. Tahltan grammar synopsis. Manuscript, Simon
Fraser University.
Blevins, Juilette. 2004. Evolutionary Phonology: The emergence of
sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bob, Tanya. 1999. Laryngeal phenomena in Tahltan. Vancouver:
University of British Columbia M.A. thesis.
Carter, Colin & Tahltan
Tribal Council. 1994. Tahltan children’s
illustrated dictionary. Dease Lake: Tahltan Tribal Council.
Greenberg, Joseph H.
1966/2005. Language universals: With
special reference to feature hierarchies. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Hardwick, Margaret. 1984. Tahltan morphology and phonology.
Toronto: University of Toronto M.A. thesis.
Hargus, Sharon. 1985. The lexical phonology of Sekani. Los
Angeles: UCLA Doctoral dissertation.
Hargus, Sharon. 2007. Witsuwit'en grammar: Phonetics, grammar,
morphology. Vancouver: UBC Press.
Hoijer, Harry. 1963. The
Athapaskan languages. Studies in the
Athapaskan languages, University of California Publications, Vol. 29, ed.
by Harry Hoijer, 1-29. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California
Press.
Huld, Martin E. 1983.
Athapaskan bears. International Journal
of American Linguistics 49.186-95.
Krauss, Michael. 1964.
Proto-Athapaskan-Eyak and the problem of Na-Dene: The phonology. International Journal of American
Linguistics 30.118-31.
Krauss, Michael. 1973.
Na-Dene. Current trends in linguistics,
Vol. 10. Linguistics in North America, ed. by Thomas A. Sebeok, 903-78. The
Hague: Mouton.
Krauss, Michael. 1979/2005.
Athabaskan tone. Athabaskan prosody,
ed. by Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice, 55-136. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Krauss, Michael & Victor
Golla. 1981. Northern Athapaskan languages. Handbook
of North American Indians. Volume 6. Subartic, ed. by June Helm, 67-85.
Washington: Smithsonian Institute.
Krauss, Michael & Jeff
Leer. 1981. Athapaskan, Eyak, and Tlingit sonorants. Alaska Native Language Centre Research Papers, Number 5.
Leer, Jeff. 1979. Proto-Athabaskan verb stem variation, part
one: Phonology. Fairbanks, Alaska: Alaska Native Language Center,
University of Alaska.
Leer, Jeff. 1985. Report on
the recommended Tahltan orthography. From the Tahltan Orthography Workshop held
at Yukon Native Language Center, Whitehorse, Yukon, July, 1985. Manuscript,
Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska.
Leer, Jeff. 1987. Navajo and
Comparative Athapaskan. The Navajo
language: A grammar and colloquial dictionary, ed. by Robert Young &
William Morgan, 264-301. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.
Leer, Jeff. 1996. The
historical evolution of the stem syllable in Gwich'in (Kutchin/Loucheux)
Athabaskan. Athabaskan language studies,
ed. by Eloise Jelinek, Sally Midgette, Keren Rice & Leslie Saxon, 193-234.
Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.
McIlwraith, Thomas. 2012. We are still Didene: Stories of hunting and
history from Northern British Columbia. Toronto: University of Toronto
Press.
Moore, Patrick J. 2002. Point of view in Kaska historical narratives
[Part 2: Kaska grammar]: Indiana University.
Nater, Hank. 1989. Some
comments on the phonology of Tahltan. International
Journal of American Linguistics 55.25-42.
Nater, Hank. 2006.
Athabascan verb stem structure: Tahltan. What's
in a verb? Studies in the verbal morphology of the languages of the Americas,
ed. by Grazyna Rowicka & Eithne B Carlin, 53-72. Utrecht: LOT.
Pierrehumbert, Janet. 2003.
Probabilistic phonology: Discrimation and robustness. Probability theory in linguistics, ed. by Rens Bod, Jennifer Hay
& Stefanie Jannedy, 177-228. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Randoja, Tiina Kathryn.
1989. The phonology and morphology of
Halfway River Beaver. Ottawa: University of Ottawa, Doctoral dissertation.
Rice, Keren. 1994. Northern
Athapaskan languages - An introduction. Manuscript, University of Toronto.
Rice, Keren. 2004. Language
contact, phonemic inventories, and the Athapaskan language family. Linguistic Typology 8.321-83.
Shaw, Patricia. 1991.
Consonant harmony systems: The special status of coronal harmony. The special status of coronals, ed. by
C. Paradis & J. F. Prunet, 125-57. New York: Academic Press.
Sheppard, Janice R. 1983. The history and values of a Northern
Athapaskan Indian village: University of Wisconsin, Doctoral dissertation.
Story, Gillian. 1975.
Tahltan: An Interpretation of Ken Hale’s Data at Telegraph Creek. Unpublished
handwritten manuscript.