Aspects of Ethiopian Komo (morpho)phonology
Manuel
Alejandro Otero
University
of Oregon
This paper presents the first comprehensive analysis
of the phonological system of Komo (Koman of Western Ethiopia), including the tone system and
morphophonological processes in the verb involving deictic directional
morphology. The consonant inventory contains plain, ejective, implosive stops
and an ejective affricate in three places of
articulation. Komo has a seven-vowel inventory with Advanced
Tongue Root (ATR) contrast in the high vowels and a typologically rare bi-directional
ATR harmony system. Komo displays anticipatory [+ATR]
harmony as well as progressive [–ATR] harmony, which, when taken together, call
into question the notion of a single “dominant” ATR feature value in an ATR
harmony system.
1
Introduction
Komo (also known as Koma, ISO 693-3
code xom)
is a little-described endangered language spoken along the Ethiopia, Sudan and
South Sudan border. Determining the population of Komo speakers at
present has been challenging for several reasons. Lewis et al.’s (2015)
estimates (10,000 in Sudan and 5,000 in Ethiopia) date back to 1976 and the
most recent Ethiopian census data from 2007, citing 15,000 ethnic ‘Komo’ in Ethiopia, groups together speakers of Komo and the related Koman language Gwama (FDCREPC 2008). In Ethiopia, speakers
of Komo and Gwama identify
ethnically as “Komo” yet recognize they speak two
mutually unintelligible languages (Yehualashet 2008, Küspert 2015). Henceforth, “Komo”,
will refer specifically to speakers of the Komo language, which can be found in two main areas in Ethiopia, the Mao-Komo special woreda (district)
of the Benishangul-Gumuz Regional State and in Gambella, the capital of the Gambella Regional State and in a nearby village.
In
the Mao-Komo special woreda, which extends westward to the border of South Sudan, the
majority of Komo speakers live in remote enclaves
scattered along the border, communicable only by footpaths. Historically,
communities residing in these borderlands, including the Komo,
have lived in a continuum of subjection and migration (Meckelburg
2015:23), the result of being traded as slaves, a practice which dates back to
the 19th century in this region (Corfield 1938:129). This was also witnessed in
the late 1800’s by the Dutch traveller Juan María Schuver, who visited Komo villages in the Sudan (James et al. 1996). The slave
trade led to cultural and social subjugation, which resulted in forced migration
and fragmentation of the people groups of the Ethio-Sudan
borderlands for decades. Slave trading of the Komo
and other minority groups of this area continued well into the 1930s (Meckelburg 2015). More recently, due to the current federal
policy towards ethnic diversity in Ethiopia, the Komo
people, along with other minority populations inhabiting remote areas of
western Ethiopia (e.g. Gwama and Opo),
have undergone resettlement to villages nearer to larger towns. This process is
ongoing and is increasing language shift in these communities to more dominant
languages.
In
Ethiopian villages, the Komo typically live among
other ethnicities and intermarriage with these groups is common. In the
Benishangul-Gumuz Regional State, Komo
often intermarry with Gwama, Oromo and to a lesser
extent, speakers of Dana.
In the Gambella State, Komo
mostly live among the Anywa, Oromo, Nuer and Opo. Komo native speakers are by
and large multilingual in Ethiopia, as they often learn the languages of the
larger people groups they are in contact with. The
contrary is almost never found. The current panorama in Ethiopia leads the
present author to suggest that Ethiopian Komo is a
highly endangered language, as many of its speakers are assimilating to
neighboring dominant cultures and languages.
There
has been ongoing controversy over the internal structure of the Koman family as well as its affiliation to the larger
Nilo-Saharan phylum. This is evidenced as far back as Corfield (1938) and
Tucker and Bryan (1966), who struggled with auto-determined and external
nomenclature for the Koman groups they encountered,
as well as the significant dialectal variation among clans speaking Koman languages.
Externally, Koman languages have traditionally been
classified within the Nilo-Saharan phylum (Greenberg 1963). Bender (1991, 1994)
places Koman coordinate with Gumuz,
Kadu, and the East Sudanic branch within what he
calls the ‘core’ group within Nilo-Saharan. By contrast, Ehret (2001)
reconstructs Koman as one of two primary splits from
Proto Nilo-Saharan.
Dimmendaal (2011), the most conservative, maintains Koman as an independent family until more extensive
grammatical information becomes available. This paper does not directly address
the external classification issue but the phonological analysis in the paper is
an important step for any subsequent studies.
The Koman languages as a whole were largely undescribed until
recently. The only comprehensive work on Komo is a
short grammatical sketch by a missionary in the Sudan (Burns 1947). Burns’
pioneering work, both in terms of phonetics/phonology and morphosyntax, laid
the groundwork for Bender (1983, 1994), who examined Komo
within a Koman perspective employing prior data and
his own field notes. Subsequently Kutsch-Lojenga
& Otero (2011) provided a brief refinement of Komo
phonology. While Negash (2015) provides a cursory
phonological analysis, the work differs significantly from the current
analysis, primarily in the vowels and tone. The major aims of this paper are to
clarify analytical anomalies and provide a comprehensive overview of the major
(morpho)phonological processes. The data for this paper come
from roughly twenty months of fieldwork spread over various trips beginning in
2011 and continuing to the present. The current database contains over 3500
roots, 35 analyzed texts and countless verb paradigms.
This
paper is organized as follows: the contrastive consonant inventory is discussed
in §2. Contrastive vowels
and ATR harmony are treated in §3.
Syllable structure and ambiguous sequences are examined in §4 and contrastive tone
melodies in monomorphemic roots and in polymorphemic inflected verbs are
discussed in §5.
Lastly, morphophonological processes in the verb are discussed in §6.
2
Consonants
The Komo contrastive consonant inventory contains 23 phonemic
consonants (Table
1).
There is a three-way contrast for plosives in the bilabial, alveolar and velar
places of articulation and a three-way contrast in manner of articulation,
which includes plain, ejective and implosive stops. This inventory is largely
consistent with previous analyses of Komo, with the
exception of the velar nasal and the glottal stop, which are of marginal
status.
|
Bilabial
|
Alveolar
|
Palatal
|
Velar
|
Glottal
|
Plosive
|
plain
|
p b
|
t d
|
|
k g
|
(ʔ)
|
ejective
|
pʼ
|
tʼ
|
|
kʼ
|
|
implosive
|
ɓ
|
ɗ
|
|
|
|
Fricative
|
|
|
s z
|
ʃ
|
|
h
|
Affricate
|
ejective
|
|
sʼ
|
|
|
|
Nasal
|
|
m
|
n
|
|
(ŋ)
|
|
Lateral
|
|
|
l
|
|
|
|
Trill/Flap
|
|
|
r
|
|
|
|
Approximant
|
|
w
|
|
y
|
|
|
Table 1. Komo contrastive consonant inventory
|
2.1 Marginally
phonemic consonants: the
velar nasal [ŋ] and the glottal stop [ʔ]
I
consider the glottal stop /ʔ/
and the velar nasal /ŋ/ marginally phonemic consonants in the inventory due to
their very limited distribution. Both the velar nasal and the glottal stop can
occur phonetically in predictable environments: the glottal stop can occur
before a word-initial vowel presumably to satisfy syllable onset requirements
and the velar nasal can surface as a homorganic nasal before a velar stop. But
there are instances where both the glottal stop and the velar nasal are
phonemically contrastive segments discussed below. Burns (1947) only mentions
the velar nasal as an allophone of the alveolar nasal assimilating to a
following velar consonant.
The
velar nasal occurs in two positions, word-internally and word-finally, where it
may or may not alternate with the alveolar nasal (Table 2).
Word internally, the velar nasal occurs in two distinct environments:
intervocalically, where it is marginally contrastive as very few near-minimal
pairs exist; or as a homorganic nasal assimilating in place to a following
velar stop across morpheme boundaries. In word-final position, the velar nasal
[ŋ] can alternate with the alveolar nasal [n] in some lexemes but not all, an
alternation that varies across different speakers. It
is difficult to know for certain whether or not the word-final velar nasal is
the result of a prior homorganic [NCvelar]
sequence from which the velar stop was lost over time. In total, there are only
about twenty instances of an unconditioned velar nasal in my data.
VŋV
|
pɛ́ŋɪ̄
|
[pʰɛ́ŋɪ̄]
|
‘flatland’
|
báŋá
|
[báŋá]
|
‘game (trad.)’
|
lǐŋí
|
[li᷅ŋí]
|
‘hill passage’
|
lāŋú
|
[lə̄ŋú]
|
‘tree (sp.)’
|
nàbɔ̀ŋà
|
[nàbɔ̀ŋà
~ nàbɔ̀ŋgà]
|
‘pelican’
|
ŋG
|
hʊ̀ngɪ̀
|
[hʊ̀ŋgɪ̀]
|
‘(they) with...’
|
zɛ̀ŋzɛ̀ŋ
|
[zɛ̀ŋzɛ̀ŋ]
|
‘stringed instrument’
|
hánkúr
|
[hə́ŋkúr]
|
‘He left (sth./s.o)’
|
ŋ#
|
wɔ̀lɔ́ŋ
|
[wɔ̀lɔ́ŋ
~ wɔ̀lɔ́n]
|
‘hare’
|
dɪ̀lɪ́ŋ
|
[dɪ̀lɪ́ŋ]
|
‘earring’
|
Table 2
Intervocalic, homorganic and word final velar nasals
|
Although
also of marginal status, the glottal stop appears to have a larger presence in
the phonological system than the velar nasal. Before discussing the contrastive
instances of the glottal stop, the predictable phonetic realization of glottal
stop is explained.
First,
there is phonetic glottal stop epenthesis before word-initial vowels. This
epenthetic glottal stop is non-contrastive and has a gradient phonetic
realization across speakers (Table
3).
Vowel initial words are not altogether common in Komo
and one possible explanation of epenthesis of the glottal stop is to satisfy
certain syllable onset requirements. Thus far there do not appear to be any
co-occurrence restrictions regarding word-initial glottal stop epenthesis and
vowel or tone.
ɔ̀ʃ
|
[ˀɔ̀ʃ]
|
‘stone’
|
āt
|
[ˀātʰ]
|
‘child’
|
ɪ̄nà
|
[ˀɪ̄nà]
|
dem
|
ʊ̀m
|
[ˀʊ̀m]
|
2pl
|
úl
|
[ˀúl]
|
‘call’
|
ɛ̄g
|
[ˀɛ̄g]
|
‘limp’
|
Table 3 Phonetic glottal stop
epenthesis in vowel-initial words
|
There
is a handful of monomorphemic verb roots that display what appears to be a
contrastive word-final glottal stop. These CVʔ verb roots
behave like CVC roots when they occur
with inflectional morphology. To illustrate, near minimal pairs inflected with the
deictic directional suffix /-í/ dd∅ are
seen in Table 4.
In this paradigm, verb roots ending in a consonant take the dd∅ suffix
/-í/ realized as [i]
before bound pronominal suffixes that only consist of a consonant (e.g. 3sg.m /-r/). By contrast, roots that end
in a vowel take the dd∅ suffix
realized as zero.
In a near minimal pair, the verb /yàʔ/ ‘vomit’ takes
the /-i/ form of the dd∅ suffix,
whereas /yà/ ‘go.sg’
takes the zero form of the suffix.
yàʔ [yàʔ]
|
‘vomit.sg’
|
yàʔ-í-r
|
[yə̀ʔír]
|
‘He vomits’
|
yà [yà]
|
‘go.sg’
|
yà-∅/í-r
|
[yār ~ yə̄r]
|
‘He goes’
|
bɔ̀ʔ [bɔ̀ʔ]
|
‘hide.sg’
|
bɔ̄-ī-r
|
[bōʔīr
~ bōīr]
|
‘He hides’
|
Table 4 Contrastive word-final
glottal stops in verbs
|
In
some roots, there is an alternation between the presence and absence of a
glottal stop, as seen in the inflected verb /bɔʔ/ ‘hide’ (Table 4). Further,
a very small portion of synchronically monomorphemic lexical roots exhibit a
phonetic intervocalic glottal stop (Table 5). In
natural speech, the intervocalic glottal stop can also be elided and is thus creating
a lack of phonological contrast for the glottal stop in this position.
bāʔɔ̄
|
[bāʔɔ̄ ~ bāɔ̄]
|
‘young woman’
|
wɛ̀ʔí
|
[wèʔí ~ wèī]
|
‘grinding stone’
|
yàʔɔ́
|
[yàʔɔ́ ~ yàɔ̄]
|
‘rainy season’
|
yàʔwɪ́
|
[yàʔwɪ́ ~ yàwɪ́]
|
‘gazelle’
|
Table 5 Noun roots with
intervocalic glottal stops
|
As
seen in Table 1, stops contrast for three points of
articulation (bilabial, alveolar and velar), as well as for four laryngeal
features (voiceless, voiced, ejective and implosive).
The allophonic realizations for these eleven plosives are discussed below.
First,
the bilabial and alveolar implosives have a limited distribution, occurring
only in word-initial position where they are realized as traditional
implosives, with ingressive air following an oral release.
Second, plain voiceless stops are phonetically aspirated in word-initial and
word-final position. Intervocalically, voiceless stops can be realized with
aspiration or they can also lenite to voiceless fricatives. Third, voiced stops
are realized with a delayed release before a pause. Aspiration is thus a key
phonetic cue that distinguishes voicing in plain stops both word-initially and
word-finally. Word-initial and word-final phonetic realizations in plain voiced
and voiceless stops are seen in Table 6.
|
Word-initial
|
Word-final
|
/p/
|
pá
|
[pʰá]
|
‘hoof, shoe’
|
kɔ̀p
|
[kʷɔ̀pʰ]
|
‘upper arm’
|
/t/
|
tùʃ
|
[tʰùʃ]
|
‘cotton, thread’
|
āt
|
[ˀātʰ]
|
‘child’
|
/k/
|
kíʃ
|
[kʰíʃ]
|
‘cocoyam’
|
sʼík
|
[sʼíkʰ]
|
‘rat’
|
/b/
|
bàr
|
[bàr]
|
‘hip, waist’
|
dɔ̀b
|
[dʷɔ̀b ~
dʷɔ̀b̚]
|
‘lion’
|
/d/
|
dɪ̀n
|
[dɪ̀n]
|
‘lake’
|
dùd
|
[dùd ~ dùd̚]
|
‘elbow’
|
/g/
|
gwà
|
[gwà]
|
‘elephant’
|
zʊ̀g
|
[zʊ̀g ~ zʊ̀g̚]
|
‘guinea fowl’
|
Table 6 Allophones of
word-initial and word-final stops and fricatives
|
Intervocalically,
it is common for the voiceless stops to lenite and weaken to fricatives (Table 7), creating
non-contrastive free variation among the allophones of /p/ ([p, pʰ,
ɸ]), /t/ ([t, tʰ])
and /k/ ([k, kʰ,
x]). Plain voiced stops can also lenite intervocalically surfacing as the
voiced fricatives and tap [β, ɾ,
ɣ]. Intervocalic
lenition of plain voiceless and voiced stops is non-categorical and gradient
across speakers, more evident in casual or fast speech.
/p/
|
pɛ̀pí
|
[pʰèpí ~ pʰèpʰí ~ pʰèɸí]
|
‘leaf’
|
/t/
|
pātí
|
[pʰə̄tí ~ pʰə̄tʰí]
|
‘liver’
|
/k/
|
sàkʊ́
|
[sàkʊ́ ~ sàkʰʊ́ ~ sàxʊ́]
|
‘grandfather’
|
/b/
|
gībā
|
[gībā ~ gīβā]
|
‘people’
|
/d/
|
mādā
|
[mādā ~ māɾā]
|
‘place’
|
/g/
|
làgá
|
[làgá ~ làɣá]
|
‘shelf’
|
Table 7 Allophones of
intervocalic plain stops
|
2.3 Lenition
and neutralization:
Implosives and ejectives
The
implosives have a limited word-initial distribution and do not display any
significant allophonic variation. As such, implosive consonants will not be
discussed further in this section. The ejectives, on the other hand, are not restricted
within the word and display both categorical and free phonetic variation.
“Categorical variation” here refers to an ejective
phoneme which has particular allophonic realizations in specific environments
and within specific types of words. Free variation refers to allophones created
via common phonetic processes, namely intervocalic lenition and word-final
articulation without a glottal release.
Word-initially,
the ejectives are always articulated with glottal release. Word-medially, the ejectives
/pʼ, tʼ, kʼ, sʼ/ can either be released,
or weaken to voiced allophones [b, d, g, z], respectively.
Word-finally, there is variation between released and unreleased ejective
allophones. The behavior suggests an intermediate process of deglottalization in
word-final position across place of articulation: the bilabial is the most
affected, followed by the alveolar and lastly the velar ejective.
Fallon
(1998:193) claims that crosslinguistically,
deglottalization of ejectives is a natural type of lenition, often resulting in
neutralization with stops. Deglottalization occurs most commonly in coda
position and it is more common for ejectives to neutralize with voiceless
stops. In the related Koman language Gwama, Goldberg (2015) describes ejective neutralization
with voiced stops in medial position and complete neutralization with both
voiced and voiceless stops in word-final position.
In Komo monomorphemic roots, intervocalic ejectives have an
audible release, clearly contrasting with other plosives, as seen in the near
minimal triplets in Table 8.
Further, speakers have very little confusion identifying intervocalic ejectives
and discriminating them from plain stops in monomorphemic roots.
/pʼ/
/b/
/p/
|
gʊ̀pʼɪ́
gùbí
kùpú
|
[gʊ̀pʼɪ́ʔ]
[gùbíʔ]
[kʰùpʰú]
|
‘stew’
‘hut’
‘cane’
|
/tʼ/
/d/
/t/
|
tʼùtʼúm
tùdúk
tútút
|
[tʼutʼum]
[tʰùdúkʰ]
[tʰútʰútʰ]
|
‘fold’
‘dry out’
‘crossroads’
|
/kʼ/
/g/
/k/
|
lɔ̌kʼɔ̀
lɔ̀gɔ́n
lūkà
|
[lɔ᷅kʼɔ̀]
[lɔ̀gɔ́n]
[lūkʰàʔ]
|
‘monkey’
‘wall’
‘basket’
|
/sʼ/
/z/
/s/
|
kìsʼísʼì
gìzá
kísí
|
[kʰìsʼísʼìʔ]
[gìzáʔ]
[kʰísíʔ]
|
‘charcoal’
‘money’
‘pocket’
|
Table 8 Plain stop and ejective
contrasts: near minimal triplets
|
In
monomorphemic noun roots, word-final ejectives are also only released (Table 9). There is no allophonic alternation with an
unreleased ejective pronunciation for the ejectives in noun roots word-finally.
/pʼ/
|
kʼʊ́pʼ
wàpʼ
|
[kʼʊ́pʼ]
[wàpʼ]
|
‘head’
‘warthog’
|
/tʼ/
|
lɛ̀tʼ
kwàntʼ
|
[lɛ̀tʼ]
[kwàntʼ]
|
‘tongue’
‘scar’
|
/kʼ/
|
ʃùmàkʼ
lɔ̀lɔ̀kʼ
|
[ʃùmàkʼ]
[lɔ̀lɔ̀kʼ]
|
‘bone’
‘brain’
|
/sʼ/
|
sʼàsʼ
yìsʼ
|
[sʼàsʼ]
[yìsʼ]
|
‘chest’
‘hide (n)’
|
Table 9 Pronunciation of word-final ejectives in Noun roots
|
Monomorphemic
verb roots, by contrast, exhibit a different pattern word-finally (Table 10).
Word-final sibilant ejectives are released [sʼ]. The
alveolar and velar ejectives /tʼ, kʼ/
are in free variation with unreleased allophones [t̚, k̚]
and the bilabial ejective /pʼ/ is unreleased [p̚].
/pʼ/
|
ʃápʼ
sʼūpʼ
wɔ́pʼ
|
[ʃáp̚]
[sʼūp̚]
[wɔ́p̚]
|
‘hit, slap’
‘dip’
‘slurp’
|
/tʼ/
|
hátʼ
ʃútʼ
mʊ̀tʼ
|
[hátʼ ~ hát̚]
[ʃútʼ ~ ʃút̚]
[mʊ̀tʼ ~ mʊ̀t̚]
|
‘stomp’
‘stuff into’
‘scoop out’
|
/kʼ/
|
kʼwàkʼ
sʼɪ̄kʼ
wàʃíkʼ
|
[kʼwàkʼ ~
kʼwàk̚]
[sʼɪ̄kʼ ~ sʼīk̚]
[wə̀ʃíkʼ ~ wə̀ʃík̚]
|
‘gossip’
‘be sour’
‘boil’
|
/sʼ/
|
hasʼ
ɓísʼ
gʊ̀sʼ
|
[hasʼ]
[ɓísʼ]
[gʊ̀sʼ]
|
‘wash’
‘be strong’
‘swallow’
|
Table 10 Pronunciation of word-final
ejectives in Verb roots
|
In
polymorphemic inflected verbs, intervocalic ejectives exhibit considerable allophony. For instance, /tʼ, kʼ/ can be realized as [tʼ, kʼ] or as unaspirated [t, k]. By contrast, /sʼ/ largely retains its glottal release, being realized as
[sʼ], though weakening to [z] also occurs.
The bilabial ejective /pʼ/ is very frequently, if not
always, realized as [b] in natural fast speech.
The data in Table 11
show the verb roots seen in Table 10
inflected with the morpheme /-í/ dd∅, followed by the person marker /-r/ 3sg.m or /-n/ 3sg.n.
|
ʃápʼ
|
[ʃə́bír]
|
‘He hits.’
|
|
hátʼ
|
[hə́tʼír ~ hə́tír]
|
‘He stomps.’
|
/pʼ/
|
sʼúpʼ
|
[sʼúbír]
|
‘He dips.’
|
/tʼ/
|
ʃútʼ
|
[ʃútʼír ~ ʃútír]
|
‘He stuffs.’
|
|
yɪ̄pʼ
|
[yɪ̄bɪ́r]
|
‘He spears.’
|
|
mʊ̀tʼ
|
[mʊ̀tʼɪ́r ~ mʊ̀tɪ́r]
|
‘He scoops out.’
|
/kʼ/
|
sʼɪ́kʼ
wàʃíkʼ
|
[sʼɪ́kʼɪ́n ~ sʼɪ́gɪ́n]
[wə̀ʃìkʼín]
|
‘It is sour.’
‘It boils.’
|
|
hásʼ
|
[hə́sʼír ~ hə́zír]
|
‘He chews.’
|
/sʼ/
|
ɓísʼ
|
[ɓísʼír ~ ɓízír]
|
‘He is strong.’
|
|
gʊ̀sʼ
|
[gʊ̀sʼɪ́r ~ gʊ̀zɪ́r]
|
‘He swallows.’
|
Table
11 Pronunciation of intervocalic ejectives in
inflected verbs
|
In
sum, word-final ejectives in monomorphemic noun roots behave distinctly from
word-final ejectives in monomorphemic verb roots. Noun roots preserve the
laryngeal feature while in verb roots, ejectives are optionally unreleased
word-finally and neutralize with voiced non-ejective allophones
intervocalically. One observation is that Komo noun
roots do not ever occur with suffixes or enclitics while verb roots are frequently
inflected with vowel-initial suffixes. The following Table 12 contains a complete distribution of the
allophones of the stops and sibilants in word-initial, intervocalic and
word-final positions.
|
#__
|
V__V
|
__#
|
|
|
#__
|
V__V
|
__#
|
/p/
|
p, pʰ
|
p, pʰ, ɸ
|
pʰ
|
|
/s/
|
s
|
s
|
s
|
/t/
|
t, tʰ
|
t, tʰ
|
tʰ
|
|
/z/
|
z
|
z
|
z
|
/k/
|
k, kʰ
|
k, kʰ, x
|
kʰ
|
|
/sʼ/
|
sʼ
|
sʼ,
z
|
sʼ
|
/b/
|
b
|
b, β
|
b̚
|
|
/pʼ/
|
pʼ
|
pʼ,
b
|
pʼ,
p̚
|
/d/
|
d
|
d, ɾ
|
d̚
|
|
/tʼ/
|
tʼ
|
tʼ,
t, d
|
tʼ,
t̚
|
/g/
|
g
|
g, ɣ
|
g̚
|
|
/kʼ/
|
kʼ
|
kʼ,
g
|
kʼ,
k̚
|
Table 12 Allophones of stops and
sibilants in word-initial, medial and final positions
|
3.1 Contrastive
vowel inventory
Komo has seven contrastive vowels /i, ɪ, ɛ, a, ɔ, ʊ, u/ (Table 13). Members of this inventory are
partially distinguished by the feature of Advanced Tongue Root (ATR), where
phonemic ATR contrast occurs in the high vowels. Three [–high, +ATR] vowels [e,
o, ə] occur on the surface as allophones of /ɛ, ɔ, a/, respectively, as a
result of anticipatory [+ATR] harmony (see §3.2 for discussion). Evidence for
the ‘basic’ status of [–ATR] is the fact that the vowels [e, o, ə] do not occur
in monosyllabic monomorphemic roots and only occur in a specific environment
which contains a [+high, +ATR] vowel /i, u/.
Burns (1947) proposes a ten-vowel inventory including seven oral vowels /i, e, ɛ, a, ɔ, o, u/ and three nasal vowels /ĩ, ɛ̃, ã/.
Nasal vowels, if they are indeed phonemic, are at best marginally phonemic and
are always followed by a nasal consonant.
|
[+ATR]
|
|
[–ATR]
|
|
+high
|
i
|
|
u
|
ɪ
|
|
ʊ
|
+high
|
-high
|
[e]
|
|
[o]
|
ɛ
|
|
ɔ
|
-high
|
|
[ə]
|
|
|
a
|
|
Table 13 Komo oral vowel inventory
(allophones in brackets)
Evidence
for a seven-vowel phonemic inventory is seen in the following (near) minimal
pairs in Table
14
where first tier contains data contrasting the front vowels and /a/ and the
second tier shows contrast in the back vowels and /a/.
/i/
|
ʃít
|
‘braid’
|
kíʃ
|
‘cocoyam’
|
yǐ
|
‘water’
|
īʃ
|
‘sleep.pl’
|
/ɪ/
|
ʃɪ́t
|
‘whistle’
|
kʼɪ̀ʃ
|
‘cheek’
|
yɪ̄
|
‘horn’
|
ɪ̄ʃ
|
‘body’
|
/ɛ/
|
ʃɛ̄t
|
‘antelope’
|
kɛ́ʃ
|
‘roast’
|
yɛ́
|
‘slice’
|
ɛ̄r
|
‘be
prepared’
|
/a/
|
ʃàtàr
|
‘be
angry’
|
kàʃ
|
‘take
out of’
|
yà
|
‘go.sg’
|
áʃ
|
‘war’
|
/u/
|
túl
|
‘gourd’
|
ʃùg
|
‘wake
up’
|
ùʃ
|
‘eat.pl’
|
tʼùmá
|
‘fist’
|
/ʊ/
|
tʊ̀l
|
‘be
tall.pl’
|
ʃʊ̀g
|
‘palm
(sp.)’
|
ʊ̄ʃ
|
‘refuse’
|
tʊ̀mà
|
‘tobacco’
|
/ɔ/
|
tɔ̀l
|
‘be
fat.sg’
|
ʃɔ̀g
|
‘leg,
foot’
|
ɔ̀ʃ
|
‘stone’
|
tɔ̄m
|
‘spear’
|
/a/
|
tálá
|
‘back’
|
ʃàk
|
‘fish
(v.)’
|
áʃ
|
‘war’
|
tàmbá
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
Table 14 Phonemic vowel contrast
in (near) minimal sets
|
A plot of the Komo vowel space reproduced here
from Olejarczuk, Otero & Baese-Berk’s
(2016) study is seen in Figure
1.
The study measured a total of 6,815 total vowel tokens in a variety of
consonantal environments extracted from 1,636 utterances produced by twelve adult male L1 Komo speakers. A total of 6,815
vowels (including those in the verbs and in the surrounding words) were
measured at the midpoint for F1and F2.
Formants were measured manually using Praat software
(Boersma 2002) and normalized using the Lobanov
procedure to account for physiological differences across the speakers (Kendall & Thomas 2010).
The ellipses in the vowel
plot indicate regions within 1 standard deviation of the means. All plots
collapse across prosodic and segmental environments. F1 was the most
reliable correlate, where [+ATR] vowels had significantly lower F1 than their
[–ATR] counterparts, which is consistent with other acoustic studies of languages
with ATR harmony (e.g. Fulop et al. 1998, Guion et
al. 2004).
Figure 1 Komo acoustic vowel space (Olejarczuk, Otero & Baese-Berk
2016)
Advanced
tongue root [ATR] harmony systems are often found in the Niger-Congo and
Nilo-Saharan languages of sub-Saharan Africa (Casali
2008:497). Casali’s (2003) typological survey of 110
languages found a direct correlation between the phonemic vowel inventory and
the type of ATR harmony system displayed. The specific criterion is whether or
not the phonemic ATR contrast is in the high vowels. Languages like Komo, which have phonemic ATR contrast in the high vowels,
overwhelmingly display [+ATR] ‘dominant-recessive’ harmony (Casali
2008:520). In this system, the feature [+ATR], considered ‘dominant’, spreads
to ‘recessive’ [–ATR] vowels in a particular phonological domain (most often
the word). Komo does in fact display this kind of [+ATR]
dominance; but I show that the overall system also allows for [–ATR] spreading.
In fact,
Komo has a highly productive and typologically
unusual ATR harmony system that displays two distinct types of ATR assimilatory
processes. The first process exemplifies [+ATR] dominant-recessive harmony and
is anticipatory (leftward spreading), occurring in monomorphemic roots as well
as in polymorphemic verbs. The second type is progressive (rightward spreading)
[–ATR] harmony, where only high [–ATR] root vowels spread the [–ATR] feature to
high [+ATR] suffix vowels. This paper summarizes Otero’s (2015) analysis and
provides acoustic evidence by Olejarczuk, Otero and Baese-Berk (2016) to support the claims of an underlying
seven vowel system and two distinct ATR harmony processes.
Komo displays two categorical ATR harmony processes that
are opposite both in terms of the ATR feature that spreads as well as the
direction of the spreading. These processes are seen primarily in inflected
verbs, as Komo lacks productive morphological
processes on the whole outside of the verb. The verb roots in Table
15
contain [–high, –ATR] vowels /ɛ,
a, ɔ/. Notice that
the root vowels remain [–ATR] when preceding a [–ATR] vowel in a suffix,
whether it’s [+high, –ATR] /ʊ/
as seen in the first set or [–high, –ATR] /a/ in the second. In these data, the
entire domain remains harmonic for the [–ATR] feature, and [–high, –ATR] root
vowels co-occur with [–ATR] suffix vowels.
V
|
Root
|
Root-dd1-3pl
|
Root-1sg
|
ɛ
|
kɛ́ʃ
|
kɛ́ʃ-ʊ́-n
|
[kɛ́ʃʊ́n]
|
‘They threshed’
|
kɛ́ʃ-á
|
[kɛ́ʃá]
|
‘I
thresh’
|
gɛ̄m
|
gɛ̄m-ʊ́-n
|
[gɛ̄mʊ́n]
|
‘We weeded’
|
gɛ̄m-á
|
[gɛ̄má]
|
‘I
weed’
|
a
|
sās
|
sās-ʊ́-n
|
[sə̄sʊ́n]
|
‘They greeted’
|
sās-á
|
[sāsá]
|
‘I greet’
|
bàb
|
bàb-ʊ́-n
|
[bàbʊ́n]
|
‘They buried’
|
bàb-á
|
[bābā]
|
‘I bury’
|
ɔ
|
gɔ̄g
|
gɔ̄g-ʊ́-n
|
[gʷɔ̄gʊ́n]
|
‘They nibbled’
|
gɔ̄g-á
|
[gʷɔ̄gá]
|
‘He nibbles’
|
kɔ́p
|
kɔ́p-ʊ́-n
|
[kʷɔ́pʊ́n]
|
‘They baled’
|
kɔ́p-á
|
[kʷɔ́pá]
|
‘I bale’
|
Table 15 [-high, -ATR] Verb roots and inflected verbs
The
first type of harmony spreads [+ATR] leftward from a [+high, +ATR] vowel /i, u/ to a [–high, –ATR ] vowel /ɛ, a, ɔ/, causing the latter to surface as [e, ə, o] [+ATR] allophones.
To illustrate [+ATR] harmony, Table
16
contains the same verb roots seen in Table
15, now inflected with suffixes containing a [+high,
+ATR] vowel /i, u/. The result is anticipatory (leftward) [+ATR] dominant
harmony, as the underlying [–high, –ATR] vowels /ɛ, a, ɔ/
in the roots become [+ATR] [e, ə, o] before [+high, +ATR] /i,
u/. Notice also that the harmony is directionally constrained as the [+ATR]
feature does not spread rightward to the 1pl.in
suffix vowel /a/ at the right edge of the word.
V
|
Root
|
Root-dd2-1pl.in
|
Root-dd∅-3sg.m
|
ɛ
|
kɛ́ʃ
|
kɛ́ʃ-úk-à
|
[kéʃúkà]
|
‘We threshed’
|
kɛ́ʃ-í-r
|
[kéʃír]
|
‘He
threshes’
|
gɛ̄m
|
gɛ̄m-úk-à
|
[gēmúkà]
|
‘We weeded’
|
gɛ̄m-í-r
|
[gēmír]
|
‘He
weeds’
|
a
|
sās
|
sās-úk-à
|
[sə̄súkà]
|
‘We greeted’
|
sās-í-r
|
[sə̄sír]
|
‘He greets’
|
bàb
|
bàb-úk-à
|
[bə̀búkà]
|
‘We buried’
|
bàb-í-r
|
[bə̄bīr]
|
‘He buries’
|
ɔ
|
gɔ̄g
|
gɔ̄g-úk-à
|
[gōgúkà]
|
‘We nibbled’
|
gɔ̄g-í-r
|
[gōgír]
|
‘He nibbles’
|
kɔ́p
|
kɔ́p-úk-à
|
[kópúkà]
|
‘We baled’
|
kɔ́p-í-r
|
[kópír]
|
‘He bales’
|
Table 16 Anticipatory [+ATR] harmony in inflected verbs
Anticipatory [+ATR] harmoy in Komo not only occurs across morpheme boundaries but it is
also evidenced in roots. Table 17
contains disyllabic monomorphemic noun and verb roots in which [+high, +ATR]
vowels /i, u/ in the second syllable spread the [+ATR]
feature value leftward to the [–high, –ATR] vowels of the first syllable.
ɛ
|
bɛ̀zí
|
[bèzí]
|
‘be thin’
|
mɛ̀tí
|
[mètí]
|
‘chase’
|
a
|
pātí
|
[pə̄tí]
|
‘liver’
|
gàbút
|
[gə̀bút]
|
‘gruel’
|
ɔ
|
kɔ́sí
|
[kósí]
|
‘bread (trad.)’
|
gɔ̀dùm
|
[gòdùm]
|
‘sow’
|
Table 17 [+ATR] harmony in monomorphemic roots
The
second ATR harmony process is what distinguishes Komo
from Casali’s (2003, 2008) typology. This process
only involves the high vowels and spreads [–ATR] rightwards from a [+high, –ATR] /ɪ, ʊ/
root vowel to a [+high, +ATR] suffix vowel /i, u/. In
Table 18, the [+high, +ATR] vowels in the dd2 and dd∅ suffixes (-úk and –í respectively) assimilate to [–ATR] after
[+high, –ATR] root vowels. Note that the harmony targets are the same suffix
morphemes (containing the same suffix vowels) that triggered [+ATR] harmony in
the environment seen in Table 18.
V
|
Root
|
Root-dd2-1pl.in
|
Root-dd∅-3sg.m
|
ɪ
|
ʃɪ́t
|
ʃɪ́t-úk-à
|
[ʃɪ́tʊ́kà]
|
‘We whistled’
|
ʃɪ́t-í-r
|
[ʃɪ́tɪ́r]
|
‘He whistles’
|
zɪ̀l
|
zɪ̀l-úk-à
|
[zɪ̀lʊ́kà]
|
‘We twisted’
|
zɪ̀l-í-r
|
[zɪ̀lɪ́r]
|
‘He twists’
|
ʊ
|
pʊ̄k
|
pʊ̄k-úk-à
|
[pʊ̄kʊ́kà]
|
‘We crossed’
|
pʊ̄k-í-r
|
[pʊ̄kɪ́r]
|
‘He crosses’
|
bʊ̀d
|
bʊ̀d-úk-à
|
[bʊ̀dʊ́kà]
|
‘We unthatched’
|
bʊ̀d-í-r
|
[bʊ̄dɪ̄r]
|
‘He unthatches’
|
Table 18 Progressive [–ATR]
harmony in inflected verbs
|
To illustrate that not only root
vowels can trigger [–ATR] spreading, the data in Table 19 show the same verb roots seen
in Table 15, now inflected
with /-ʊ́/ dd1 followed by /-í/ 2sg. There are two phonetic outcomes in
this case. The vowel /ʊ/ of the dd1 morpheme
can either be realized as a bilabial glide [ʷ], presumably to avoid vowel
hiatus, or /ʊ/ can be completely elided. What is relevant to the discussion of
harmony is that in both cases, the [–ATR] feature of /ʊ/ is spread rightward to
the following underlyingly [+ATR] vowel /i/ in the 2sg morpheme, causing it to surface as [–ATR] [ɪ]. Notice
that in these data, the domain of harmony surfaces as entirely [–ATR].
V
|
Root
|
Root-dd1-2sg
|
ɛ
|
kɛ́ʃ
|
kɛ́ʃ-ʊ́-í
|
[kɛ́ʃʷɪ́ ~ kɛ́ʃɪ́]
|
‘He
threshed’
|
gɛ̄m
|
gɛ̄m-ʊ́-í
|
[gɛ̄mʷɪ́ ~ gɛ̄mɪ́]
|
‘He
weeded’
|
a
|
sās
|
sās-ʊ́-í
|
[sāsʷɪ́ ~ sāsɪ́]
|
‘He greeted’
|
bàb
|
bàb-ʊ́-í
|
[bābʷɪ́ ~ bābɪ́]
|
‘He buried’
|
ɔ
|
gɔ̄g
|
gɔ̄g-ʊ́-í
|
[gɔ̄gʷɪ́ ~ gɔ̄gɪ́]
|
‘He nibbled’
|
kɔ́p
|
kɔ́p-ʊ́-í
|
[kɔ́pʷɪ́ ~ kɔ́pɪ́]
|
‘He baled’
|
Table
19 CV[-high,
-ATR]C verb roots inflected with dd1 /-ʊ́/ and 2sg /-í/
|
Table 20 displays roots containing [+high,
+ATR] vowels inflected with the same morphology as the verbs in Table 19, [–ATR] /-ʊ́/ dd1 followed by [+ATR] /-í/ 2sg. The data in Table 20 show how [–ATR] spreads
rightwards from /ʊ/ to /i/ as seen in Table 19, but crucially the [–ATR]
feature does not spread leftwards to
the potential [+high, +ATR] root vowels /i, u/. In
this environment, the phonological word remains disharmonic for the ATR
feature.
V
|
Root
|
Root-dd∅-2sg
|
i
|
gìz
|
gìz-ʊ́-í
|
[gìzʷɪ́
~ gìzí]
|
‘Enter
(towards me)!’
|
kíʃ
|
kíʃ-ʊ́-í
|
[kíʃʷɪ́
~ kíʃɪ́]
|
‘Harvest
(towards me)!’
|
u
|
wúsʼ
|
wúsʼ-ʊ́-í
|
[wúsʷɪ́
~ wúsɪ́]
|
‘Close
(towards me)!’
|
dùb
|
dùb-ʊ́-í
|
[dùbʷɪ́
~ dùbɪ́]
|
‘Dig
(towards me)!’
|
Table 20 CV[+high,
+ATR]C verb roots inflected with dd1 /-ʊ́/ and 2sg
/-í/
|
To
summarize, both ATR harmony processes in Komo are
directionally constrained: [+ATR] harmony spreads leftward while [–ATR] harmony
spreads rightward. Furthermore, the domain of harmony is limited to the word
and only spreads to the vowel of the adjacent target syllable as seen in (1) where [+ATR] harmony occurs
in the inflected verb and does not spread to the potential vowel targets in the
preceding or the following word.
(1)
|
[hàr
|
ʃə̄bīr
|
kʼáw]
|
|
hàr
|
ʃāpʼ-ī-r
|
kʼáw
|
|
3sg.m
|
hit-dd∅-3sg.m
|
dog
|
|
‘He hits/is hitting a/the dog.’
|
The data in Table
16
exemplify Casali’s (2003, 2008) typology for African
languages with ATR contrast in the high vowels, displaying what can be
characterized as anticipatory [+ATR] dominant harmony. He observes that one
common characteristic of [+ATR] dominant harmony is the presence of underlyingly
[+ATR] affixes (typically suffixes) that cause [–ATR] root vowels to assimilate
to [+ATR]. In Komo, the [+ATR] vowels /i, u/ seen in the dd2
and dd∅ suffixes
behave as prototypical triggers of
[+ATR] harmony in one environment, yet non-prototypically become targets of [–ATR] spreading in another. While Casali (2008) finds that it is
extremely common for languages with this vowel inventory to display [+ATR]
dominance, he also notes that these languages may have a few cases of [–ATR] spreading. Casali refers to this as
“superficial [–ATR] dominance", and argues that it is only found in limited
circumstances within a [+ATR]-dominant system. Baković
(2000) calls such exceptions “dominance reversals”, and
claims that they are unproductive and restricted to a few idiosyncratic
morphemes. The facts of Komo resist such
"rule-and-exception" accounts; both ATR processes are extremely
productive and appear to be phonologically conditioned. This apparent symmetry
calls into question the typological validity of the notion of ATR “dominance”.
4 Syllable structure
A
template of the Komo syllable is seen in Figure
2.
I discuss the unambiguous syllable types (CV and CVC), complex codas (CVNC) and
vowel-initial syllables in §4.1. Glides are treated in §4.2, by first describing
phonetically conditioned labialization and palatalization then moving onto
complex onsets (CGV, CGVC) in §4.3. Lastly, consonant clusters
within polysyllabic words and across syllables types are examined in §4.4.
Figure 2 Komo syllable template
By far the most common and most
unambiguous syllable type in monomorphemic words are CV and CVC (Table 21). Every consonant in the
phonemic inventory can occur as an onset of a word-initial CV/CVC syllable.
Any consonant, with the exception of implosives /ɓ, ɗ/ and the glottal
fricative /h/ can occur as codas of a word-final CVC syllable. Syllable nuclei
in the CV and CVC shapes are always vocalic elements that function as tone
bearing units.
Complex codas in monomorphemes are restricted to
having a nasal consonant as the first of a two consonant NC coda. CVNC
syllables are restricted to word-final position while CV and CVC syllables can
occur both word-medially and word-finally.
CV
|
ɓî
|
‘eye’
|
zā.gà
|
‘name’
|
CVC
|
bùʃ
|
‘belly’
|
mɔ̀l.tʼá
|
‘satchel’
|
CVNC
|
ʃʊ̄nkʼ
tɔ̄nsʼ
|
‘tendon,
vein’
‘squirrel
|
Table 21 Common unambiguous syllable types
|
Vowel initial syllables with V and
VC shapes are also attested, though at a much lower frequency than CVC and CV
syllable types.
In word-initial position a vowel-initial syllable may occur with a phonetic
glottal stop [ˀV], as discussed in §2.1. This suggests that glottal stop
epenthesis may be a strategy employed to derive a more common CV(C) syllable
shape from a vowel-initial syllable to satisfy a phonotactic constraint of a
consonant in syllable onset position word-initially.
In Komo, glides can occur both phonetically and phonologically,
and this has led to differing analyses in prior research. Burns (1947:3)
asserts that all of the 23 consonants in the phonemic inventory can be
labialized or palatalized word-initially but it is unclear whether he is
positing phonologically-contrastive consonant phonemes, consonant allophones,
or sequences of two separate consonants. Bender (1983:261), proposes
non-phonemic labialized allophones [pʷ, tʷ, sʼʷ, ʃʷ,
kʷ,
gʷ,
kʼʷ]
restricted to word-initial position. In discussing complex onsets, Negash (2015) mixes predictable, phonetically-conditioned
labialized and palatalized consonants with phonological consonant-glide (CG)
onsets. I first discuss the predictable phonetic result of coarticulation from
the following vowel onto a preceding consonant (i.e. “allophones” of the
consonants) and then move on to complex onsets of the CG type. Lastly, I
discuss ambiguous sequences.
There
is non-contrastive labialization and palatalization occurring on word-initial
consonants only in environments containing the mid vowels /ɛ, ɔ/. Phonetic
labialization of a word-initial consonant occurs when followed by the mid back
round vowel [ɔ, o],
and similarly conditioned palatalization occurs with consonants preceding [ɛ, e]. Consonants which
do not participate in these processes are nasals, liquids and semi-vowels.
Examples of phonetic palatalization and labialization are in Table
22.
bɛ̌s
|
[bʸɛ᷅s ~ bɛ᷅s]
|
‘dream
|
pɔ́g
|
[pʷɔ́g̥]
|
‘back’
|
dɛ̀sʼɛ̀
|
[dʸɛ̀sʼɛ̀ ~ dɛ̀sʼɛ̀]
|
‘insect
(sp.)’
|
dɔ̀ʃ
|
[dʷɔ̀ʃ]
|
‘stand.sg’
|
sʼɛ́m
|
[sʸɛ́m ~ sʼɛ́m]
|
‘be
sweet’
|
sɔ́k
|
[sʷɔ́kʰ]
|
‘sit.pl’
|
kɛ́m
|
[kʸɛm ~ kɛ́m]
|
‘pierce’
|
kɔ̀gɔ́
|
[kʷɔ̀gɔ́]
|
‘music’
|
tɛ́l
|
[tʸɛ́l ~ tɛ́l]
|
‘nail’
|
tʼɔ́ʃ
|
[tʼʷɔ́ʃ]
|
‘roast’
|
Table 22 Phonetic
palatalization and labialization
examples
|
It
is important to underscore that the above-mentioned processes are strictly the
phonetic result of coarticulation. There is no evidence for phonemic contrast
between “labialized” and non-labialized consonants before the vowel /ɔ/ ([ɔ, o]) (e.g. “kʷɔ”
vs. “kɔ”)
and likewise is there no contrast between “palatalized” and non-palatalized
consonants before /ɛ/
[ɛ, e] (e.g. “sʼʸɛ”
vs. “sʼɛ”).
Prior descriptions of Komo have transcribed some of
the sequences in Table
22 as if they were phonemic Cw/Cy
sequences.
Thus the predictable phonetic labialization and palatalization do not have any
bearing on the canonical CV and CVC syllable types.
While
the previous section showed how two particular vowels cause phonetic
labialization and palatalization in word-initial consonants, I interpret these
as a single consonantal phoneme in a simple syllable onset. The only complex
onsets in Komo consist of an initial consonant
followed by either a labiovelar approximant /w/ or a palatal glide /y/.
Furthermore, the vowel /a/ is always the syllable nucleus of a Cy onset. Cw onsets occur primarily with /a/ as the nucleus, though
there are few exceptions discussed below.
The near minimal pairs in Table 23
provide evidence for complex onsets, again restricted to word-initial
syllables.
twáy
|
‘hunger’
|
tà
|
cop
|
ʃwà
|
‘dance’
|
ʃá
|
‘eat.sg (soft food)’
|
syángā
|
‘fish (sp.)’
|
sángà
|
‘tomorrow’
|
zyālá
|
‘fish (sp.)’
|
zālázālá
|
‘tree (sp.)’
|
kwál
|
‘steal’
|
kál
|
‘pour from small vessel’
|
gwà
|
‘elephant’
|
gà
|
‘termite hill’
|
kʼwà
|
‘rock hyrax’
|
kʼà
|
‘eat.pl (hard food)’
|
Table 23 CV ~ CGV near minimal
pairs
|
Complex
onsets containing a labio-velar approximant (CwV) are
headed by all attested phonemic stops and fricatives except for the bilabial
implosive /ɓ/ and
the voiceless bilabial ejective /pʼ/. The CyV sequence is much more limited and the attested C-glide
sequences in word-initial position are seen in Table 24.
Cw
|
|
|
Cy
|
|
|
tw
|
twáy
|
‘hunger’
|
by
|
byànzā
|
‘morning
star’
|
dw
|
dwāg
|
‘drag’
|
sʼy
|
sʼyán
|
‘tinderstick’
|
kw
|
kwál
|
‘steal’
|
zy
|
zyālá
|
‘fish,
sp.’
|
kʼw
|
kʼwàtʼ
|
‘tick’
|
ky
|
kyāl
|
‘be
silent’
|
gw
|
gwà
|
‘elephant’
|
gy
|
gyàlá
|
‘roast
(over fire)’
|
ɗw
|
ɗwàkàɗwàkà
|
‘kidney’
|
|
|
|
tʼw
|
tʼwàrárà
|
‘African
thrush’
|
|
|
|
sw
|
swà
|
‘be
big’
|
|
|
|
sʼw
|
sʼwàngá
|
‘mushroom,
sp.’
|
|
|
|
ʃw
|
ʃwà
|
‘dance’
|
|
|
|
ʔy
|
ʔyámʊ̄n
|
‘seed’
|
|
|
|
Table 24
Word-initial CGlide sequences with /a/ nuclei
|
A peculiar phenomenon is the
existence of a word-initial, phonetically palatalized glottal stop [ʔʸ]. I interpret this as a glottal stop followed by a
palatal glide sequence occurring before the vowel /a/, which patterns
consistently with the data in Table 24.
The palatalized glottal stop is found in three words, [ʔʸámʊ̄n]
‘seed’, [ʔʸāmàʔ] ‘sorghum’ and the verb [ʔʸákʰ] ‘go.dd2’,
which appears to be a form of the motion verb ‘go.sg’ /yà/ that has been
lexicalized with the Deictic Directional morpheme /-úk/
(dd2). A full paradigm for the
defective verb [ʔʸák] is seen in Table 25. Notice that the palatalized
glottal onset surfaces phonetically in the 1st person forms when the vowel of
the second syllable is [a], but not in the rest of the paradigm when /a/
surfaces as [ə] as a result of anticipatory [+ATR] harmony.
1sg
|
ʔʸák-Ø-á
|
[ʔʸákʰá]
|
‘I go/ went.’
|
2sg
|
ʔʸák-Ø-í
|
[ˀə́kʰí]
|
‘You go/went.’
|
3sg.m
|
ʔʸák-í-r
|
[ˀə́kʰír]
|
‘He goes/went.’
|
3sg.f
|
ʔʸák-í-pʼ
|
[ˀə́kʰíp̚]
|
‘She goes/went.’
|
3nh
|
ʔʸák-í-n
|
[ˀə́kʰín]
|
‘It goes/went.’
|
1pl.in
|
ʔʸák-Ø-à
|
[ʔʸákʰà]
|
‘We go/went.’
|
1pl.ex
|
ʔʸák-Ø-án
|
[ʔʸákʰán]
|
‘We go/went.’
|
2pl
|
ʔʸák-í-m
|
[ˀə́kʰím]
|
‘You all go/went.’
|
3pl
|
ʔʸák-í-n
|
[ˀə́kʰín]
|
‘They go/went.’
|
Table 25 Full paradigm of verb ʔyák ‘go’
|
Taking
word-initial consonant-glide sequences into account, three additional syllable
types can be derived (Table
26).
These syllable types pattern with the unambiguous types Table
21
in that CG onset syllables allow a simple coda CGVC and a complex CGVNC coda.
Further, CG onset syllables distribute like CV syllables in that CGV and CGVC
can occur word-medially and CGVNC can only occur word-finally. There are no
instances of a word final CGV syllable in which the glide element is a palatal
glide /y/.
CGV
|
gwà
|
‘elephant’
|
ʃwà
|
‘dance’
|
CGVC
|
dwàg
|
‘raise, lift’
|
sʼyán
|
‘tindersticks’
|
CGVNC
|
kwàntʼ
|
‘scar’
|
byànsʼá
|
‘wasteland’
|
Table 26 C-glide complex onset
syllable types
|
There
is a handful of high-frequency words that don’t follow the general patterns
described above for the distribution of syllables within a word. The exceptions
are words that largely contain a high, front vowel in the nucleus, a contour
tone and a complex Cw onset. Table
27
contains words with complex onsets followed by a vowel with a level tone (CGV)
as compared to words with a vowel with a contour tone (CGV̌). Notice that the
nuclei of the proposed CGV syllable types can contain a high, front vowel /i, ɪ/ as well as /a/.
CGV [i, ɪ]
|
CGV̌[i, ɪ]
|
CGV̌[a]
|
|
kwɪ̀
|
‘chief’
|
kwǐ
|
‘fall (intr.)’
|
twǎ
|
‘mud’
|
zwɪ̀
|
‘antelope’
|
ʃwǐ
|
‘beer’
|
kwǎlá
|
‘throwing stick’
|
pwī
|
‘burnt area’
|
ʃwɪ̌
|
‘rope, vine’
|
kwǎngá
|
‘pumpkin’
|
kwíʃí
|
‘skin (v.)’
|
|
|
|
|
Table 27 Word-initial CG onsets before high, front vowels
|
While
examining Semitic languages of Ethiopia, Goldenberg (2013:77-78) observes
“[...] a general tendency of Ethiopian languages to decompose u into its components, viz. labiality or
rounding and syllabicity (ʷ +V), and
then to lose concomitant labiality when following a non-labializable
consonant (C + V), but retain it when following a labializable
(kʼ, x, g or k) consonant (Cʷ+V).” Comparative data from related Koman
languages reveals the vocalic origin of certain ambiguous sequences. For
instance, a CGV̌ sequence containing a high, front vowel quality in the nucleus
in Komo, e.g. ʃwǐ ‘beer’ has cognates containing /u/ as
a vowel nucleus in related languages- sū in Uduk, ʃʊ́l in Highland Gwama and sùd̪ in Dana,
which suggests a vocalic origin for some synchronic Komo
labialized Cw onsets, specifically in this case when
occurring with a contour tone and a high, front vowel nucleus. Further, Cw sequences in Komo occur
frequently with “labializable” /k, g, kʼ, ʃ/ as the initial consonant and less so with /s, t, sʼ/. Further investigation and comparative evidence is needed.
The
previous sections described consonant clusters in tauto-syllabic
onsets and tauto-syllabic codas. Hetero-syllabic sequences
of two distinct consonants (C1C2), are somewhat rare in
isolated monomorphemic words.
Table 28 contains hetero-syllabic
consonant sequences (C1C2) across syllables within
monomorphemic words.
On the whole nasals and liquids form the initial consonant in a sequence
followed by sibilants. Initial stops are rare and limited to the few examples
in the table, some of which are place names that may not be of Komo origin. No medial or final clusters containing the
following consonants in first position were found: /p, pʼ, ɓ, d, tʼ,
ɗ, k, g, kʼ, z, h, w, y, ʔ/.
p
|
pk
|
ʃàp.kā.rá
|
‘Shapkara’
|
b
|
bʃ
|
zɛ́b.ʃɛ̀r
|
‘Zebsher’
|
t
|
tk
|
gwàt.kár
|
‘forked
pole’
|
sʼ
|
sʼk
|
bīsʼ.kī.rí
|
‘bird, sp.’
|
s
|
st,
sk
|
bǎs.tɛ̄
|
‘biscuit’
|
ʃ
|
ʃm,
ʃk
|
túʃ.kír
|
‘heart/organ’
|
m
|
mp,
mb, mpʼ, mt, ms,
|
dàm.bɛ́r
|
‘syphilis’
|
n
|
nt,
nd, ntʼ, ng, nkʼ, ns, nz, nsʼ
|
hàn.kʼá
bàn.tʼʊ́
|
‘bow (n.)’
‘enset’
|
l
|
lb,
ltʼ, lk, lg, lkʼ, lm
|
gǎl.kʼún
dɔ̀l.bá
|
‘clavicle’
‘now’
|
r
|
rk,
rg, rkʼ, rs, rʃ, rm,
rw
|
gàr.wɛ́ʃ pǎr.ʃá
|
‘partridge’
‘horse’
|
Table 28 Consonant
sequences in monomorphemic words
|
5
Tone
Komo displays three level tones as well as rising and
falling contour tones. Overall, tone is quite stable in Komo:
there is no evidence of downstep or downdrift in prosodic words or even across longer sentences
and virtually every underlying tone and tone melody surfaces faithfully. The functional
load of tone is moderate in the lexicon, more present in the verbal system
where verbs can be subcategorized into classes by their tonal behavior.
Further, many verb roots can also be tonally suppletive
in number. In §5.1, I briefly discuss
contrastive tone melodies in monomorphemic nouns and then move on to verbs in §5.2, where the discussion focuses
on outlining the general patterns for verb tone classes and highlighting some
anomalies in the system.
Burns (1947) claims there are four level tones in Sudanese Komo but I find no evidence,
either phonetic or phonological, to support this in Ethiopian Komo. The current analysis proposes three level tones:
(L)ow, (M)id and (H)igh. Phonemic contrast for the
three level tones in monosyllabic words is seen in the near minimal
monomorphemic triplets in Table 29.
L
|
sʼàsʼ
|
‘chest’
|
kɔ̀p
|
‘upper
arm’
|
M
|
sʼɔ̄sʼ
|
‘be
small.pl’
|
kɔ̄pʼ
|
‘antelope
(sp.)’
|
H
|
sʼásʼ
|
‘ant
(sp.)’
|
kɔ́p
|
‘bale
out’
|
Table 29 Near minimal
monomorphemic level tone triplets
|
There is ample phonological data to show contrast among the three level
tones (cf. Appendix B). The acoustics also confirm these findings as the F0
values of the three level tone categories are significantly distinct from each
other. The mean and standard deviation of the fundamental frequencies for each
of the three level tones were measured from a total of 6,815 vowel tokens
extracted from 1,636 utterances produced by twelve male L1 Komo
speakers. The data were taken from Olejarczuk, Otero
& Baese-Berk’s (2016) study following the same
methodological protocols seen in the acoustic measurements of the vowels (see §3). F0 values were normalized by
dividing each token’s F0 by the speaker’s mean F0 across all vowels. Results
are seen in Figure 3.
Figure 3 Fundamental frequency measures for the three level
tones
Komo also displays (R)ising and (F)alling contour tones
in monosyllabic monomorphemic roots as well as across morpheme boundaries in
polymorphemic words, which can be monosyllabic or polysyllabic. In
polymorphemic words, the contour tones can be analyzed as consisting of
phonemic level tones. There does not appear to be more than one contrastive or
rising tone in monomorphemic words. To be explicit, there are no minimal pairs
of two falling tones (e.g. HL vs. HM) nor for rising tones (e.g. LM vs. LH).
yɪ̄
|
M
|
‘axe
|
|
ɓɪ̀
|
L
|
‘mountain’
|
|
yǐ
|
R
|
‘water’
|
|
ɓî
|
F
|
‘eye’
|
|
dā
|
M
|
q
|
|
bā
|
M
|
dem
|
|
dâ
|
F
|
‘mother’
|
|
bǎ
|
R
|
‘father’
|
|
Table 30 Contour tone contrasts
in monomorphemic roots
|
5.1 Tone
in nouns
All
of the possible combinations of the three level tones, which comprise a total
of nine tone patterns, are exhibited in disyllabic nouns (Table 31).
There do not appear to be any restrictions with regard to consonants and tonal
combinations as both voiced and voiceless consonants occur before all three of
the tone levels. Out of the data in Table 31,
the word-level tone combinations L.M, L.H, M.L, M.H, H.L, H.M can be
extrapolated.
|
L
|
M
|
H
|
L
|
kʼɔ̀lɔ̀ ‘hand’
tʊ̀mà ‘tobacco’
|
màkɔ̄ ‘poison’
gàbūt ‘gruel’
|
mɔ̀ltʼá ‘satchel’
tʼùmá ‘fist’
|
M
|
ɓɪ̄sʼà ‘crocodile’
kʊ̄mà ‘path’
|
kāmā ‘maize’
ʃākā ‘herd’
|
tʼɛ̄tʼíʃ ‘pot’
ɓāmít ‘woman’
|
H
|
kʼásʼɪ̀ ‘ground’
dʊ́zɛ̀ ‘pipe’
|
kákā ‘thorn’
pʼɪ́pʼɪ̄ʃ ‘ant’
|
sʼílít ‘thatch’
párá ‘hut’
|
Table 31 Tone
melodies in disyllabic nouns
|
Monomorphemic
words with more than two syllables are not common on the whole in Komo. The tonal patterns found in trisyllabic
monomorphemic nouns are varied and some are rarer than others. Nonetheless, 26
of the 27 possible combinations are attested, with the exception of H.M.M (Table 32).
|
__.__.L
|
__.__M
|
__.__H
|
L.L
|
L.L.L
kàgʊ̀mà ‘molar’
|
L.L.M(.H)
gàlɪ̀mʊ̄tʼá ‘cuckold’
|
L.L.H
pùlàní ‘dust’
|
L.M
|
L.M.L
tànānà ‘heel’
|
L.M.M
mɪ̀ʃīgī ‘mourning’
|
L.M.H
gàrgārɪ́ ‘lizard
(sp.)’
|
L.H
|
L.H.L
gʊ̀tʊ́lʊ̀ ‘chin’
|
L.H.M
lɪ̀tʼɪ́mān ‘point’
|
L.H.H
kàʃílá ‘kindling’
|
M.L
|
M.L.L
tʼātɔ̀mɔ̀ ‘door’
|
M.L.M
bālàlākʼ ‘centipede
(sp.)’
|
M.L.H
māyɪ̀gɪ́ ‘brother
in law’
|
M.M
|
M.M.L
dīsʼīnà ‘pimple’
|
M.M.M
bābālūwā ‘fish
(sp.)’
|
M.M.H
zālālá ‘fish
(sp.)’
|
M.H
|
M.H.L
kālúwà ‘fish eagle’
|
M.H.M
būbáʃɛ̄ ‘nightjar’
|
M.H.H
bātʼʊ́rsá ‘woodpecker’
|
H.L
|
H.L.L
gʊ́làlà ‘hawk’
|
H.L.M
pɪ́kʊ̀lɪ̄ ‘hornbill’
|
H.L.H
búlbùtú ‘bird
(sp.)’
|
H.M
|
H.M.L
gʊ́lɪ̄là ‘tree (sp.)’
|
H.M.M
–
|
H.M.H
písākɔ́ ‘firefly’
|
H.H
|
H.H.L
kándúlà ‘mud
wasp’
|
H.H.M
kánásʊ̄ ‘seven’
|
H.H.H
sɪ́kɪ́ná ‘intestines’
|
Table 32 Tone patterns in trisyllabic nouns
|
To
generalize across the tone patterns on trisyllables, if a sequence of two
identical tones followed by a distinct tone (e.g. L.L.H) can be analyzed as
underlyingly L.H in autosegmental terms following the
Obligatory Contour Principle (OCP), then a total of 12 contrastive patterns can
be analyzed (L.M.L, L.M.H, L.H.L, L.H.M, M.L.M, M.L.H, M.H.L, M.H.M, H.L.M,
H.L.H, H.M.L, H.M.H).
Tone
has a heavier functional load in verbs, which is seen on the root level as well
as in polymorphemic inflected verbs. Monosyllabic (C)V(C) verb roots are the
predominant shape, though disyllabic CVCV(C) also occur.
Tone
in Komo is rather stable overall, meaning that
surface tone is almost always equivalent to the underlying tone, but there is
an exception. In order to illustrate this, I examine monosyllabic verb roots.
Monosyllabic verb roots fall into strict verb classes based on two criteria:
the underlying tone of the verb root and how the root behaves tonally when
inflected with dd and bound pronominal suffixes. The vast
majority of monosyllabic verb roots occur with all three level tones (L, M, H).
Every finite verb must take one of three Deictic Directional (dd) suffixes, all of which contain a H
tone vowel (Table
33).
This suffix is followed by a bound pronominal suffix, most often realized as a
consonant, which indexes an argument on the verb.
The resulting shape of a verb with a monosyllabic root but also inflecting for
a single argument is CVCV(C), containing two vocalic nuclei, each of which is a
tone bearing unit.
dd∅
|
dd1
|
dd2
|
/-í/
|
/-ʊ́/
|
/-úk/
|
Table 33 Deictic-Directional suffixes
|
Four
tone classes emerge when examining monosyllabic roots across the three dd paradigms. This is specifically due to
the behavior of a particular subset of L tone roots when occurring with dd∅. Table 34 schematizes the tone classes by examining the
underlying tone of monosyllabic verb roots as compared to the disyllabic
inflected forms across the three dd paradigms
when inflecting a single argument. Class L1, M and H roots
(L, M and H tones respectively), retain their root tone when inflected across
the three dd paradigms. Class L2 roots
also have a L tone when inflected with dd1
and dd2 but crucially when
inflected with dd∅, the
expected L.H pattern surfaces as M.M as seen in the shaded area in Table 34.
Class
|
Root
Tone
|
ddØ
root-sfx
|
dd1
root-sfx
|
dd2
root-sfx
|
L1
|
L
|
L-H
|
L-H
|
L-H
|
L2
|
L
|
M-M
|
L-H
|
L-H
|
M
|
M
|
M-H
|
M-H
|
M-H
|
H
|
H
|
H-H
|
H-H
|
H-H
|
Table 34 Tone patterns in
inflected verbs indexing a single argument
|
Table 35 provides Komo data to illustrate the tone
analysis schematized in Table 34. The tone classes are indicated in the first
column, followed by the root tone of the verb and a verb root from each class.
The last four columns contain verbs inflected across the three deictic
directional paradigms indexing a single argument (employing the 2pl suffix /-m/). Notice that the
underlying L tone root in kɛ̀d ‘push’ (Class L2) surfaces as M.M when inflected with dd∅ as indicated in the
shaded portion.
Class
|
Tone
|
Root
|
dd∅ /-í/
|
dd1 /-ʊ́/
|
dd2 /-úk/
|
Gloss
|
L1
|
L
|
dùm
|
dùmím
|
dùmʊ́m
|
dùmkúm
|
‘You all hit’
|
L2
|
L
|
kɛ̀d
|
kēdīm
|
kɛ̀dʊ́m
|
kèdkúm
|
‘You all push(ed)’
|
M
|
M
|
tɔ̄n
|
tōním
|
tɔ̄nʊ́m
|
tōnkúm
|
‘You all pass(ed)’
|
H
|
H
|
ɓʊ́g
|
ɓʊ́gɪ́m
|
ɓʊ́gʊ́m
|
ɓʊ́gːʊ́m
|
‘You all wait(ed)’
|
Table 35 Class I-III verbs
indexing 2pl across the three Deictic
Directional paradigms
|
The
tone classes also hold when a verb indexes two arguments (Subject and Object),
and ultimately creates a trisyllabic word. Surface
tone behavior follows the patterns outlined in Table 34.
Class L2 verbs are M.M in the dd∅
paradigm. Consider the following data that show the verb kàn ‘bury’ indexing two arguments on the verb: 3sg.m acting upon 1sg /-āk/, which is underlyingly
M tone. Notice that the dd∅ form
(shaded) surfaces as M.M(.M) while dd1 and
dd2 are both L.H(.M).
dd∅
|
kàn-í-r-āk
bury-dd∅-3sg.m-1sg
|
[kə̄nīrāk]
|
M.M.M
|
‘He
buries/ed me’
|
dd1
|
kàn-ʊ́-r-āk
bury-dd1-3sg.m-1sg
|
[kànʊ́rāk]
|
L.H.M
|
‘He
buried me’
|
dd2
|
kàn-úk-r-āk
bury-dd2-3sg.m-1sg
|
[kə̀nkúrāk]
|
L.H.M
|
‘He
buried me’
|
Table 36 Class L2 verb root indexing two arguments
in the three dd paradigms
|
The data show that not all L
tone verb roots surface as L (as Class L2 roots surface as M when
occurring with dd∅),
and not all dd∅
surface as H (as dd∅
surfaces as M with Class L2 roots). Therefore, not only does the
behavior of class L2 roots occurring with dd∅ necessitate
more complex underlying tones, but the underlying tones of the remaining
classes must be reexamined as well as these roots do not change when inflected
with dd∅.
While an in-depth analysis of tone is beyond the scope of this paper, what is important for the
current discussion of surface tone patterns is that there are four tone
classes. These four tone classes hold and remain contrastive in other aspects
of Komo verb tonology.
Disyllabic
verbs also occur in the lexicon and like disyllabic nouns, all of the nine
possible tone combinations are exhibited (Table
37).
Disyllabic verbs inflect like monosyllabic verbs, taking directional and
person-indexing suffixes.
L.L
|
hɛ̀rɛ̀tʼ
|
‘crunch (e.g. bone with teeth)’
|
L.M
|
lìlī
|
‘sink’
|
L.H
|
gwàrásʼ
|
‘scratch quickly’
|
M.L
|
ʃɛ̄lɛ̀l
|
‘be wall-eyed’
|
M.M
|
pālāk
|
‘break off (e.g. cracked wood)’
|
M.H
|
tʊ̄mpʊ́ʃ
|
‘be bent’
|
H.L
|
sʼɔ́gɔ̀t
|
‘pinch quickly (with fingernails)’
|
H.M
|
gɛ́līm
|
‘be crisscrossed’
|
H.H
|
kʼɔ́lɪ́sʼ
|
‘pinch’
|
Table 37 Tone patterns in
disyllabic verbs
|
5.3 Tonal
suppletion in verbs: Number distinction
A
large portion of verb roots, largely if not exclusively intransitive verbs, are
tonally suppletive in number. The distinction is
realized across the behavior of the four tone classes (even if not evident by
the tone of the root alone), as not all L tone roots behave similarly when
inflected. Thus, one particular tone class marks singular subjects while an
opposing class marks plural subjects. Taking into
account the four tone classes, nine of the ten possible combinations attested
are seen in Table 38.
The lack of an M (sg)/ L1
(pl)
combination may be due to insufficient data rather than a system-internal
restriction. Interestingly there is no instance of an opposition within the L
class of verbs (L1 / L2), which may suggest greater
category affinity.
Tone Class
|
|
|
sg
|
pl
|
sg
|
pl
|
|
L1
|
M
|
zàr
|
zār
|
‘walk fast’
|
L2
|
M
|
kùt
|
kūt
|
‘be short’
|
L1
|
H
|
wà
|
wá
|
‘be broken’
|
L2
|
H
|
pɛ̀sʼ
|
pɛ́sʼ
|
‘laugh’
|
M
|
L1
|
---
|
---
|
---
|
M
|
L2
|
kʊ̄n
|
kʊ̀n
|
‘be bent’
|
M
|
H
|
ɛ̄r
|
ɛ́r
|
‘be prepared’
|
H
|
L1
|
pɛ́l
|
pɛ̀l
|
‘be red’
|
H
|
L2
|
ɓísʼ
|
ɓìsʼ
|
‘be strong’
|
H
|
M
|
wásʼ
|
wāsʼ
|
‘feel pain’
|
Table 38 Monosyllabic verbs: sg/pl tone class suppletion
|
Disyllabic
verb roots also display tonal suppletion in number as
seen in Table
39.
Number distinctions on the root level and/or tonally suppletive
number on the root level in verbs is also attested in related Koman languages (e.g Opo, Smolders 2017).
Root tone
|
|
|
|
sg
|
pl
|
sg
|
pl
|
|
L.L
|
L.H
|
yìlàt
|
yìlát
|
‘be dizzy, swerve’
|
L.L
|
M.H
|
bʊ̀bàn
|
bʊ̄bán
|
‘be aslant’
|
L.L
|
H.H
|
kɔ̀nɔ̀n
|
kɔ́nɔ́n
|
‘snore’
|
L.H
|
M.H
|
zùgúm
|
zūgúm
|
‘rinse mouth’
|
L.H
|
H.H
|
pàlí
|
pálí
|
‘flirt, chat’
|
H.M
|
M.H
|
zɪ́lɪ̄n
|
zɪ̄lɪ́n
|
‘have lines’
|
H.H
|
L.L
|
pʼátʼá
|
pʼàtʼà
|
‘be white’
|
H.H
|
L.H
|
hápá
|
hàpá
|
‘fail, be absent’
|
Table 39 Disyllabic verbs: sg/pl tone suppletion
|
6
Morphophonological
processes
This section
examines the particular behavior of the Deictic Directional morpheme -úk (dd2) and its allophones [-úk /ʊ́k,
-kú /kʊ́, -k, -ʷk].
Recall that a finite verb takes bound pronominal suffixes following the Deictic
Directional suffix. The particular allomorph of -úk (dd2) is largely conditioned
by the shape of the following bound pronominal. When there is no bound
pronominal morphology on the verb, dd2 always
surfaces as -úk.
This is seen in an Auxiliary construction, which contains a fully inflected
auxiliary verb followed by a verb root inflected with a Deictic Directional [aux-bp-(bp)-(bp) V-dd].
In this construction, seen in (2)-(4), the dd2 morpheme always surfaces as either [-uk] or [-ʊk], regardless if the verb root
is consonant or vowel-final.
(2)
|
ʊ́-ná-g-ápʼ-ɪ̄
|
ʃɛ̀r-úk
|
|
int-1sg-ben-3sg.f-3sg.nh
|
sweep-dd2
|
|
‘I will have swept it for her.’
|
(3)
|
bāʃ-g-ɪ̀n
|
sɪ̀g-ʊ́k
|
|
neg-1sg-3sg.nh
|
hear-dd2
|
|
‘I haven’t heard it.’
|
(4)
|
bāʃ-g-ɪ̀n
|
ʃá-úk
|
|
neg-1sg-3sg.nh
|
eat.sg-dd2
|
|
‘I haven’t eaten it.’
|
Process rules for deriving
allomorphs of /-úk/ are seen in Table 40. Metathesis of /-úk/
occurs before consonant-initial suffixes (i), and
vowel-deletion or residual labialization occurs before a suffix containing /a/
that is either (H)igh or (M)id tone (ii).
i.
|
-úk →
-kú / __ +C
|
ii.
|
-úk →
-k ~ -ʷk / __+a (H or M tone)
|
iii.
|
-úk →
-úk (elsewhere)
|
Table 40 Process rules for -úk (dd2)
Table 41 contains a full paradigm of a
consonant-final verb ìʃ
‘sleep’ occurring with the Deictic Directional morpheme /-úk/
(dd2) inflected with bound
pronominal suffixes indexing the subject. When the bound pronominal suffix
consists of a consonant, -úk
surfaces as [-kú] as seen in 3sg.m/f, 3nh, 2pl and 3pl. Presumably, -úk metathesizes to avoid a sequence of three consonants in these
cases. When the bound pronominal suffix following /-úk/
contains a vowel that is M or H tone, -úk surfaces
as [-k] (1sg, 2sg, 1pl.ex). The [-k] allomorph also alternates with
[-ʷk] before a mid
or high tone /-a/ suffix in 1sg and
1pl.ex. The phonologically
unpredictable behavior of /-úk/ is seen when the
verb is marked with 1pl.in. In
this case, /-úk/ always surfaces as [-úk]. Whether or not this has to do with the fact that the
1pl.in morpheme is low tone -(n)à
or whether there is a diachronic explanation remains to be investigated, though
there is no other evidence of tone or tone melodies constraining segmental
structure in Komo.
1sg
2sg
3sg.m
3sg.f
3nh
1pl.in
1pl.ex
2pl
3pl
|
ìʃ-úk-á
ìʃ-úk-í
ìʃ-úk-r
ìʃ-úk-pʼ
ìʃ-úk-n
īʃ-úk-à
īʃ-úk-án
īʃ-úk-m
īʃ-úk-n
|
[ìʃká ~ ìʃʷká]
[ìʃkí]
[ìʃkúr]
[ìʃkúp̚]
[ìʃkún]
[īʃúkà]
[īʃkán ~ īʃʷkán]
[īʃkúm]
[īʃkún]
|
‘I slept’
‘You slept’
‘He slept’
‘She slept’
‘It slept’
‘We slept’
‘We slept’
‘You all slept’
‘They slept’
|
Table 41 Paradigm of ɪʃ ‘sleep’
inflected with the dd2 suffix /-úk/
Further evidence of the
allomorphic behavior of /-úk/ is seen in open syllable
roots (Table 42). Notice that the bilabial
offglide is present in 1sg in the
verbs [ʃóʷká] ‘I sewed’ and [ɗīʷká]
‘I was angry’, but not [bʷɔ̀ká] ‘I hid’, where the
initial consonant is phonetically labialized. When the verb is inflected with –(n)à 1pl.in,
the vocalic element in /-úk/ can assimilate
to the vowel of the root and a contour tone can emerge if the vowel of the root
is not H tone. When inflected with -r
3sg.m, /-úk/
metathesizes to [-kú].
1sg
|
ʃɔ́-úk-á
|
[ʃóʷká]
|
H.H
|
‘I sewed’
|
ɗī-úk-á
|
[ɗīʷká]
|
M.H
|
‘I was angry’
|
bɔ̀-úk-á
|
[bʷɔ̀ká]
|
L.H
|
‘I hid’
|
1pl.in
|
ʃɔ́-úk-à
|
[ʃóːkà]
|
Hː.L
|
‘We sewed’
|
ɗī-úk-à
|
[ɗīúkà]
|
MH.L
|
‘We were angry’
|
bɔ̀-úk-à
|
[bʷo᷅kà]
|
LM.L
|
‘I hid’
|
3sg.m
|
ʃɔ́-úk-r
|
[ʃókúr]
|
H.H
|
‘He sewed’
|
ɗī-úk-r
|
[ɗīkúr]
|
M.H
|
‘He was angry’
|
bɔ̀-úk-á
|
[bʷòkúr]
|
L.H
|
‘He hid’
|
Table 42 Allomorphs of /-úk/ on open
syllable verb roots
When
the final consonant of a verb root is a voiced stop or voiced fricative, a
following contiguous voiceless velar stop of the metathesized dd2 suffix [-kú]
becomes voiced (Table 43). Notice that the bilabial nasal can also
cause voicing assimilation but the alveolar nasal, lateral and trill do not.
/b/
|
bàb-úk-r
|
[bə̀bgúr]
|
‘He
buried/planted’
|
/d/
|
kɛ̀d-úk-r
|
[kèdgúr]
|
‘He
pushed’
|
/g/
|
tɔ́g-úk-r
|
[tógːúr]
|
‘He
tasted’
|
/z/
|
gìz-úk-r
|
[gìzgúr]
|
‘He entered’
|
/m/
|
dùm-úk-r
|
[dùmkúr ~ dùmgúr]
|
‘He
punched’
|
/n/
|
kán-úk-r
|
[kə́ŋkúr]
|
‘He
buried (s.o)’
|
/r/
|
ʃɪ́r-úk-r
|
[ʃɪ́ɾkʊ́r]
|
‘He
hung’
|
/l/
|
pʼɛ́l-úk-r
|
[pʼélkúr]
|
‘He
was white’
|
Table 43 Voicing of /úk/ dd2 in
verbs
Verb
roots ending in ejectives behave similarly to those ending in voiced stops.
Word-medially, ejectives are realized as voiced stops and cause the adjacent
voiceless velar stop of the [-k(u)] allomorph to become voiced (Table
44).
The sibilant ejective is realized as a voiceless ejective, and as such, does
not participate in the voicing assimilation process.
/pʼ/
|
ʃápʼ-úk-r
kʼápʼ-úk-á
|
[ʃə́bgúr]
[kʼábgá]
|
‘He hit’
‘I chopped’
|
/tʼ/
|
mʊ̀tʼ-úk-r
hátʼ-úk-á
|
[mʊ̀dgʊ́r]
[hádgá]
|
‘He scooped’
‘I flattened (sth.)’
|
/sʼ/
|
pɛ̀sʼ-úk-r
bàsʼ-úk-á
|
[pèsʼkúr]
[bàsʼká]
|
‘He laughed’
‘I tossed (sth.)’
|
Table 44 Ejective-final verb roots inflected with /-úk/ dd2
In §2.3 I discussed the distinct
phonetic behavior of ejectives in monomorphemic noun and verb roots. Word-final
ejectives are realized with glottal release in noun roots as opposed to verb
roots. Intervocalically, the ejective phonemes are realized with a glottal
release in monomorphemes. But in polymorphemic verbs
with inflectional morphology, the ejective and voiced stop contrast can often
be neutralized. There also appears to be a cline in terms of how the ejectives
are realized. The sibilant ejective /sʼ/ is almost
always realized as an ejective, followed by the
alveolar and velar ejectives /tʼ, kʼ/
which alternate with voiced stops. The bilabial /pʼ/
has a wider range of allophones and has neutralized with /b/ word-medially in
inflected verbs. This peculiar phonetic behavior of ejectives, specifically the
bilabial ejectives, is also reported for related Koman
languages.
6.2 Consonant
gemination in verbs: dd2 /-úk/
Consonant length is not a
contrastive phonemic feature in Komo
nor has it been reported in any other Koman language
to date. However, heterosyllabic consonant
gemination, a rare phenomenon in Komo, can occur
within stems and across morpheme boundaries and is the result of
morphophonological processes, namely metathesis, elision and partial
reduplication which conspire such that the resulting surface forms contain a
geminate consonant. The full word forms containing “geminate” consonants can
ultimately contrast with non-geminate forms as minimal or near minimal pairs.
Gemination in verbs can be seen in two environments: when a verb root ending in
a velar stop is inflected with the /-úk/ dd2 suffix, and in verb stems that
employ partial reduplication to code the pluractional.
The data in Table
41 show that certain morphophonological
processes occur in verbs inflected with the suffix /-úk/
(dd2). When the inflected verb is
formed by a root with a final velar consonant (C)VK, the result of these
processes can create a sequence of contiguous velar stops, which in turn, is
pronounced as a single geminate consonant. If other environmental factors, such
as tone and vowel harmony align, length can be the only contrastive feature
between inflected verbs (with roots that end in a velar stop). The data in Table 45 show pairs of verbs that contrast only in length. Notice
that the vowel has elided in the forms inflected with the dd2 morpheme /-uk/.
The result is a sequence of velar stops, which results in contrastive length
when compared to the same verb inflected with dd∅.
pʊ̄k-Ø-á
|
cross-ddØ-1sg
|
[pʊ̄kā]
|
‘I cross (river)’
|
pʊ̄k-úk-á
|
cross-dd2-1sg
|
[pʊ̄kːá]
|
‘I crossed (river)’
|
sɔ́k-Ø-án
|
sit.pl-ddØ-1pl.ex
|
[sʷɔ́kán]
|
‘We sit’
|
sɔ́k-úk-án
|
sit.pl-dd2-1pl.ex
|
[sʷɔ́kːán
~ sókːán]
|
‘We sat’
|
ɓʊ́g-Ø-á
|
wait-ddØ-1sg
|
[ɓʊ́gá]
|
‘I wait’
|
ɓʊ́g-úk-á
|
wait-dd2-1sg
|
[ɓʊ́gːá]
|
‘I waited’
|
Table 45 Velar consonant gemination in verbs inflected with /-úk/ dd2 compared to dd∅
Similar contrast in length is
seen in some verbs inflected for 3sg.m (Table 46). In these data, verb roots ending
in a velar stop inflected with /-úk/ dd2 contrast in length with those
inflected with /-ʊ́/ dd1. Note
that the stem vowels do not surface identically for the ATR feature in ‘He sows’. In the pairs, ‘He crosses’ and ‘He waited’, the only
phonological contrast is length. In these examples, the [−ATR] feature of the
root vowel spreads to the [+ATR] /u/ vowel of the dd2 morpheme /-úk/, causing the
vowel to surface as [ʊ́]. As such the sequences contain identical vowels and
the pairs only contrast in length.
yɛ̀k-ʊ́-r
|
sow-dd1-3sg.m
|
[yɛ̀kʊ́r]
|
‘He
sows (towards me)’
|
yɛ̀k-úk-r
|
sow-dd2-3sg.m
|
[yèkːúr]
|
‘He
sows (towards you)’
|
pʊ̄k-ʊ́-r
|
cross-dd1-3sg.m
|
[pʊ̄kʊ́r]
|
‘He
crosses (towards me)’
|
pʊ̄k-úk-r
|
cross-dd2-3sg.m
|
[pʊ̄kːʊ́r]
|
‘He
crosses (towards you)’
|
ɓʊ́g-ʊ́-r
|
wait-dd1-3sg.m
|
[ɓʊ́gʊ́r]
|
‘He
waited (at another location)’
|
ɓʊ́g-úk-r
|
wait-dd2-3sg.m
|
[ɓʊ́gːʊ́r]
|
‘He
waited (and left)’
|
Table 46 Velar consonant gemination in verbs inflected with /-úk/ dd2
compared to /-ʊ́/ dd1
6.3 Consonant
gemination in verbs: Pluractional
Pluractional
derivations can be employed to express actions as
happening to many subjects or to many objects, or it can describe events as
occurring repeatedly or habitually (Newman 1990). As such, the
pluractional is commonly considered a property that codes verbal number or rather,
multiplicity of a verbal event. In Komo, the
pluractional is expressed by morphological derivation which involves a partial
reduplication of the verb root. Monomorphemic monosyllabic verb roots in Komo have a CV or CVC shape.
In the bare form (without any inflectional morphology), roots with a CVC shape
reduplicate the final root consonant, after an epenthetic high [−ATR] vowel [ɪ̄].
Those roots with a CV shape reduplicate the first (i.e. only) consonant,
followed by [ɪ̄] (Table 47).
Root shape
|
Partial reduplication
|
Example
|
Gloss
|
C1VC2
|
C1VC2-ɪC2
|
gàs
→ gàs-ɪ̄s
|
‘celebrate’ → ‘celebrate
profusely’
|
C1V
|
C1VC1-ɪ
|
yà
→ yà-yɪ̄
|
‘go.sg → ‘wander aimlessly’
|
Table 47 Pluractional derivation
The newly derived pluractional stem
is able to inflect regularly with Deictic Directional morphology and bound
pronominal suffixes. When the pluractional stem is inflected, deletion of the
epenthetic vowel /ɪ̄/ occurs. As a result of the deletion, two identical
consonants occur in sequence, pronounced as a geminate. In (5a) the derived
pluractional verb is in the bare form following an auxiliary, as compared to (5b)
where gemination occurs since the epenthetic vowel [ɪ̄] has deleted (but not
before spreading the [−ATR] feature to the morpheme /-í/
dd∅).
(5)
|
a.
|
bāʃ-í-r
|
yìs-ɪ̄s
|
[yìsɪ̄s]
|
b.
|
yìs-ɪ̄s-í-r
|
[yìsːɪ́r]
|
|
|
neg-ddØ-3sg.m
|
circle-redup.plu
|
|
|
circle-redup.plu-ddØ-3sg.m
|
|
|
|
‘He doesn’t circle around
(continually).’
|
|
‘He circles around
(continually).’
|
Contrast
between geminate and singlelton consonants involving
the pluractional derivation is seen in Table 48 with the nominalizing enclitic =āgɪ́.
lɔ̀l=āgɪ́
|
[lɔ̀lāgɪ́]
|
‘to pat’
|
wàd=āgɪ́
|
[wādāgɪ́]
|
‘to turn (intr.)’
|
lɔ̀l-ɪ̄l=āgɪ́
|
[lɔ̀lːāgɪ́]
|
‘translation’
|
wàd-ɪ̄d=āgɪ́
|
[wàdːāgɪ́]
|
‘to swivel round and round’
|
Table 48 Gemination in nominalized verbs
7
Conclusion
This
paper is the first comprehensive examination of the core phonology of the
Ethiopian variety of Komo, an endangered language of
the Koman family. One notable feature of the Komo consonant system is the apparent deglottalization of
ejectives, which results in voiced obstruents
word-medially and unreleased obstruents word-finally.
Surface contrast in consonant length, a feature that is not considered
phonemically contrastive in the language, arises from morphophonological
processes in the verb. Komo displays a phonemic
seven-vowel inventory with ATR contrast in the high vowels, which is supported
by acoustic measurements of over 6,000 vowels taken from twelve native speakers.
One of the most unusual aspects of Komo phonology is
the bi-directional ATR harmony system. The Komo system
questions the notion of a single “dominant” ATR feature within an ATR harmony
system, as the same vocalic elements that are triggers of anticipatory [+ATR] spreading in one domain can also be
targets of progressive [–ATR] spreading
in another.
References
Baković, Eric. 2000. Harmony,
dominance and control. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University dissertation.
Bender, M. Lionel. 1971. The languages of Ethiopia: A new
lexicostatistic classification and some problems of diffusion. Anthropological
Linguistics 13(5). 165-288.
Bender, M. Lionel. 1983. Proto-Koman
phonology and lexicon. Afrika und Übersee
66(2). 259-297.
Bender, M. Lionel. 1991. Sub-classification of Nilo-Saharan.
In M. Lionel Bender (ed.), Proceedings of the 4th Nilo-Saharan Linguistics
Colloquium, 1-36. Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag.
Bender, M. Lionel. 1994. Comparative Komuz grammar. Afrika
und Übersee 77(1). 31-54.
Boersma, Paul. 2002. Praat, a
system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot
international 5(9/10). 341-345. Online: http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/.
Burns, Samuel J. 1947. Notes toward a grammar of the Koma language. London: Sudan Interior Mission, MS.
Casali, Roderic F. 2003. [ATR] value
asymmetries and underlying vowel inventory structure in Niger-Congo and
Nilo-Saharan. Linguistic Typology 7(3). 307-382.
Casali, Roderic F. 2008. ATR harmony
in African languages. Language and Linguistics Compass 2(3). 496-549.
Corfield, Frank D. 1938. The Koma.
Sudan Notes and Records 21(1).
123-165.
Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. 2011. Historical
linguistics and the comparative study of African languages. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Ehret, Christopher. 2001. A Historical-comparative
reconstruction of Nilo-Saharan. Köln: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.
Fallon, Paul D. 1998. The synchronic and diachronic
phonology of ejectives. Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University dissertation.
Federal Democratic Commission Republic of Ethiopia
Population Census (FDCREPC). 2008. Summary and Statistical Report of the
2007 Population and Housing Census. Addis Ababa, Ethiopia.
Fulop, Sean, Ethelbert Kari, &
Peter Ladefoged. 1998. An acoustic study of the
tongue root contrast in Degema vowels. Phonetica
55. 80-98.
Goldberg, Justin. 2015. Ejective neutralization in Gwama. Paper presented at the 12th Nilo-Saharan Linguistics
Colloquium, Nairobi, Kenya.
Goldenberg, Gideon. 2013. Semitic languages: Features,
structures, relations, processes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1963. The languages of Africa. Bloomington: Indiana University.
Grottanelli, Vinigi
L. 1946. Materiali di lingua Coma. Rassegna
di Studi Etiopici 5. 122-155.
Guion, Susan G., Mark W. Post, & Doris L. Payne. 2004.
Phonetic correlates of tongue root vowel contrasts in Maa.
Journal of Phonetics 32(4). 517-542.
Hellenthal, Anne-Christie, & Constance
Kutsch-Lojenga. 2011. Phonology/Orthography statement
for the Gwama language. Addis Ababa: MS.
James, Wendy, Gerd Baumann, & Douglas Hamilton Johnson
(eds.). 1996. Juan Maria Schuver’s travels in
North-East Africa, 1880-1883. London: The Hakluyt Society.
Kebebw, Lemi.
2010. A grammatical description of Opo. Addis
Ababa: University of Addis Ababa MA thesis.
Kendall, Tyler, & Erik R Thomas. 2010. Vowels: Vowel
manipulation, normalization, and plotting in R. R package, version 1.
Kievit, Dirk, & Erika Robertson.
2012. Notes on Gwama grammar. Studies in African
Linguistics 41(1). 39-97
Killian, Don. 2015. Topics in Uduk
phonology and morphosyntax. Helsinki: University of Helsinki dissertation.
Küspert, Klaus-Christian. 2015. The Mao
and Komo languages in the Begi–Tongo area in Western Ethiopia: Classification,
designations, distribution. Linguistic Discovery 13(1). 1-63.
Kutsch Lojenga,
Constance & Manuel A. Otero. 2011. Phonology/Orthography statement for the Komo language. Addis Ababa: SIL Ethiopia, MS.
Lewis, Paul M., Gary F. Simons, & Charles D. Fennig. 2015. Ethnologue:
Languages of the World, Eighteenth Edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL
international.
Meckelburg, Alexander. 2015. Slavery,
emancipation and memory: Exploratory notes on western Ethiopia. International
Journal of African Historical Studies 48(2). 345-362.
Negash, Tesfaye. 2015. Documentation
and grammatical description of Kwom. Addis Ababa:
Addis Ababa University dissertation.
Newman, Paul. 1990. Nominal and verbal plurality in
Chadic. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Olejarczuk, Paul, Manuel A. Otero, & Melissa Baese-Berk. 2016. Production and perception of the [ATR] vowel system in Ethiopian Komo. Paper presented at the 171st Meeting of the Acoustical
Society of America, Salt Lake City, UT.
Olejarczuk, Paul, Manuel A. Otero,
&Melissa Baese-Berk. in prep. Acoustic correlates of unique bidirectional [ATR] system:
The case of Ethiopian Komo
Otero, Manuel A. 2013. ATR
Harmony in Komo [xom].
Paper presented at the 44th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, Georgetown, D.C.
Otero, Manuel A. 2015. [+ATR] dominant vowel harmony except
when it’s not? Evidence from Ethiopian Komo. In Ruth
Kramer, Elizabeth C. Zsiga, & One Tlale Boyer (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 44th
Annual Conference on African Linguistics, 212-220. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Otero, Manuel A. 2016. Reconstructing pronominal morphology
in Koman languages. Paper presented at the 47th
Annual Conference on African Linguistics, Berkeley, CA.
Payne,
Doris L., & Manuel A. Otero. 2016.
Deictic (Associated-)Motion and the tense-aspect domain: Evidence from Nilotic
& Koman. Paper presented at the Syntax of the
World’s Languages 7, Workshop on Associated Motion, Mexico City, Mexico.
Smolders, Joshua. 2017. A Wordlist of T'apo (Opuuo):
Version 0.1. Addis
Ababa: SIL Ethiopia.
Theis, Joachim. 1995. Nach der Razzia: Ethnographie
und Geschichte der Koma.
München: Trickster Verlag.
Tucker, Archibald Norman, & Margaret Arminel
Bryan. 1966. Linguistic Analysis: The Non-Bantu Languages of North-Eastern
Africa. London: Oxford University Press.
van Silfhout, Marijke. 2013. Opo: Towards a phonology. Leiden: Leiden
University MA thesis.
Yehualashet, Teshome.
2008. A brief phonological description and nomenclature of Kwama/
Komo (A Nilo-Saharan language). Addis Ababa:
University of Addis Ababa, MS.
Appendix A: Evidence
for consonant contrast
p : b
|
initial
|
pá
|
‘shoe,
sandal’
|
bà
|
‘be
wide’
|
pàm
|
‘touch,
feel’
|
bám
|
1sg.poss
|
pút
|
‘plaster’
|
būt
|
‘grasshopper
(sp.)’
|
pít
|
‘vagina’
|
bìt
|
‘discard’
|
intervocalic
|
púpúʃ
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
bùbūʃ
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
ʃìpá
|
‘knife’
|
yībā
|
‘man’
|
final
|
ùp
|
‘bathe’
|
ùb
|
‘feed’
|
tʼàp
|
‘spit
(v.)’
|
táb
|
‘kick’
|
p : pʼ
|
initial
|
pɛ̀l
|
‘untie’
|
pʼɛ́l
|
‘be red’
|
pàsʼ
|
‘soak’
|
pʼàʃ
|
‘tree
(sp.’
|
intervocalic
|
púpúʃ
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
nàpʼúʃ
|
‘soft,
silky’
|
final
|
kɔ̀p
|
‘upper
arm’
|
kɔ̀pʼ
|
‘eland
antelope’
|
ɪ́p
|
‘drink.sg’
|
yɪ̄pʼ
|
‘spear,
inject’
|
p :
ɓ
|
initial
|
pàsʼ
|
‘soak’
|
ɓàsʼ
|
‘be
hot’
|
pú
|
‘decay,
rot’
|
ɓú
|
‘be
pregnant’
|
p : m
|
initial
|
pʊ́ʃ
|
‘sand’
|
mùʃ
|
‘draw
water’
|
pá
|
‘shoe’
|
mà
|
‘do’
|
intervocalic
|
kùpú
|
‘cane’
|
kūmú
|
‘clitoris’
|
dúpɛ̀
|
‘skirt
(trad.)’
|
dùmɛ̀
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
final
|
páp
|
‘defecate’
|
pàm
|
‘touch
|
ʃɛ́p
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
ʃɛ́m
|
‘stuff,
pack in’
|
p : w
|
initial
|
pūd
|
‘wind’
|
wùd
|
‘fox
(sp.)’
|
pàg
|
‘swim’
|
wàg
|
‘jeer,
slander’
|
intervocalic
|
yīpāl
|
‘young
man’
|
yīwáy
|
‘healer
|
final
|
káp
|
‘move
fire’
|
kʼáw
|
‘dog’
|
pʼ : b
|
initial
|
pʼàʃ
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
bàʃ
|
‘blood’
|
pʼīn
|
‘ashes’
|
bīní
|
2sg.poss
|
intervocalic
|
gʊ̀pʼɪ́
|
‘stew’
|
gùbí
|
‘house,
hut’
|
final
|
bɪ́pʼ
|
3sg.f.poss
|
bɪ̀b
|
‘cow’
|
pʼ : ɓ
|
initial
|
pʼísʼ
|
‘rub
hands together’
|
ɓísʼ
|
‘be
strong.sg’
|
pʼàʃ
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
ɓàsʼ
|
‘be
hot’
|
b : ɓ
|
initial
|
bà
|
‘be
wide’
|
ɓà
|
‘nape’
|
bɔ̀l
|
‘stretch’
|
ɓɔ́l
|
‘be
good’
|
b : m
|
initial
|
bùl
|
‘drum’
|
mùl
|
‘hair’
|
intervocalic
|
kʊ́bá
|
‘pillow’
|
kʊ̄mà
|
‘path,
road’
|
sʼābà
|
‘traditional
healing’
|
sʼāmà
|
‘bird
(sp.)’
|
final
|
dàb
|
‘be
fragrant’
|
dàm
|
‘honey’
|
bàb
|
‘bury’
|
bàm
|
‘move
together’
|
b : w
|
initial
|
būt
|
‘grasshopper
(sp.)’
|
wǔt
|
‘ostrich’
|
final
|
dàb
|
‘be
fragrant’
|
dàw
|
‘baboon’
|
m : n
|
initial
|
màʃ
|
‘marriage’
|
nàsʼ
|
‘grind
(wet)’
|
intervocalic
|
ámà
|
‘there’
|
ánà
|
1sg.inclusive
|
final
|
tɔ̄m
|
‘spear’
|
tɔ̄n
|
‘pass,
overtake’
|
súm
|
‘pound
in’
|
sùn
|
‘point
at, show’
|
m : w
|
initial
|
mādā
|
‘place’
|
wādá
|
‘fox
(sp.)’
|
mālà
|
‘saliva’
|
wàlá
|
‘hide
(of animal)’
|
final
|
dàm
|
‘honey’
|
dàw
|
‘baboon’
|
t : d
|
initial
|
tūt
|
‘return,
reply’
|
dùt
|
‘elbow’
|
tɛ́n
|
‘slap,
strike’
|
dɛ̀n
|
‘count,
read’
|
intervocalic
|
wātá
|
‘be
like, similar to’
|
wādá
|
‘fox
(sp.)’
|
final
|
būt
|
‘grasshopper
(sp.)’
|
bʊ̀d
|
‘unthatch’
|
t : tʼ
|
initial
|
tā
|
‘be,
exist’
|
tʼā
|
‘mouth,
language’
|
tín
|
‘fight,
war (v)’
|
tʼìn
|
‘stretch’
|
intervocalic
|
pàtàgɪ́
|
‘to
crawl’
|
pʼàtʼàgɪ́
|
‘
to be
white, clean’
|
final
|
kàt
|
‘convince,
lobby’
|
kʼátʼ
|
‘be
easy’
|
dɔ̀t
|
‘ask’
|
dɔ̀tʼ
|
‘squat’
|
t : ɗ
|
initial
|
táb
|
‘kick’
|
ɗàb
|
‘adhere’
|
tɛ́
|
‘sun’
|
ɗɛ́
|
‘one’
|
tíʃ
|
‘sing
and play’
|
ɗìʃ
|
‘be
near’
|
t : n
|
initial
|
tɛ́g
|
‘not acknowledge.sg’
|
nɛ́k
|
‘Hammerkopf’
|
tā
|
‘be,
exist’
|
ná
|
‘feel
hopeless’
|
final
|
kát
|
‘cause trouble’
|
kàn
|
‘bury’
|
t : r
|
initial
|
tɪ́t
|
‘roughen
(stone)’
|
rɪ̀tá
|
‘hyena’
|
intervocalic
|
kàtàkàtà
|
‘whisper’
|
kàrá
|
‘fish
(sp.)’
|
final
|
bát
|
‘duck
(sp.)’
|
bàr
|
‘lower
back’
|
būt
|
‘grasshopper
(sp.)’
|
bùr
|
‘ventilate,
fan’
|
t : l
|
initial
|
tūs
|
‘push’
|
lùs
|
‘hide
sth.’
|
tútút
|
‘crossroads’
|
lùtút
|
‘come
free and fall’
|
intervocalic
|
lītī tɔ̄m
|
‘shooting
star’
|
līlí
|
‘sink
(v.)’
|
final
|
bìt
|
‘toss’
|
bɪ̀l
|
‘root’
|
t : sʼ
|
initial
|
ták
|
‘surpass’
|
sʼák
|
‘burn’
|
tásʼ
|
‘smith’
|
sʼásʼ
|
‘ant
(sp.)’
|
intervocalic
|
dɪ̀tɪ̄n
|
dem.dist
|
dīsʼīn
|
‘pimple’
|
final
|
wǔt
|
‘ostrich’
|
wúsʼ
|
‘shut’
|
dɔ̀t
|
‘ask’
|
dɔ̀sʼ
|
‘suck’
|
tʼ : d
|
initial
|
tʼàʃ
|
‘salt’
|
dàʃʊ́
|
‘snake
(sp.)’
|
tʼútʼ
|
‘freckle,
spot’
|
dùd
|
‘elbow’
|
intervocalic
|
kátʼāgɪ́
|
‘be
soft’
|
kádán
|
‘fish
(sp.)’
|
final
|
tʼútʼ
|
‘freckle,
spot’
|
dùd
|
‘elbow’
|
hátʼ
|
‘clear,
flatten’
|
hàd
|
‘pull’
|
tʼ : ɗ
|
initial
|
tʼʊ́sʼ
|
‘choke’
|
dʊ̄sʼ
|
‘urinate’
|
tʼíʃ
|
‘finely
grind’
|
ɗíʃ
|
‘be
near.pl’
|
tʼ : sʼ
|
initial
|
tʼʊ́sʼ
|
‘choke’
|
sʼʊ́sʼ
|
‘scratch’
|
tʼádà
|
‘bake’
|
sʼábà
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
final
|
dɔ̀tʼ
|
‘squat’
|
dɔ̀sʼ
|
‘suck’
|
hátʼ
|
‘clear,
flatten’
|
hásʼ
|
‘ruminate,
chew cud’
|
d : ɗ
|
initial
|
dàb
|
‘be fragrant’
|
ɗàb
|
‘adhere’
|
dɛ̌
|
‘thing’
|
ɗɛ́
|
‘one’
|
dɔ̀l
|
‘cry
out’
|
ɗɔ̀
|
‘carry
many things’
|
d : n
|
initial
|
dɪ̀
|
dem.prox.1
|
nɪ̀
|
dem.prox.2
|
dɔ̄kɔ̄
|
‘potato
(sp.)’
|
nɔ̀gɔ́
|
‘worm
(sp.)’
|
intervocalic
|
dàdāgɪ́
|
‘to
complain’
|
dànāgɪ́
|
‘to
growl’
|
final
|
dàd
|
‘refuse
sth.’
|
dàn
|
‘menstrual
cycle’
|
bʊ̀d
|
‘unthatch’
|
bʊ́n
|
3pl.poss
|
d : r
|
initial
|
dɪ̄dà
|
‘lance,
spear’
|
rɪ̀tá
|
‘hyena’
|
dɔ̄kɔ̄
|
‘potato
(sp.)’
|
rɔ̀kɔ́n
|
‘corner’
|
intervocalic
|
dɪ̄dà
|
‘lance,
spear’
|
dìrà
|
‘fish
(sp.)’
|
kādán
|
‘fish
(sp.)’
|
kàrá
|
‘fish
(sp.)’
|
final
|
ùd
|
‘hit’
|
ùr
|
‘be
barren’
|
hàd
|
‘pull’
|
hàr
|
3sg.m
|
d : l
|
initial
|
dàm
|
‘honey’
|
làm
|
‘strain
(e.g. food)’
|
dʊ̀
|
‘inhabit
(temporarily)’
|
lù
|
‘sprout
(v.)’
|
intervocalic
|
mādā
|
‘place’
|
mālà
|
‘saliva’
|
final
|
bʊ̀d
|
‘unthatch’
|
bùl
|
‘drum’
|
tɔ̄d
|
‘abort’
|
tɔ̄l
|
‘be
fat.pl’
|
r : l
|
initial
|
rɔ́kɔ̀n
|
‘corner,
angle’
|
lɔ̀gɔ́n
|
‘wall’
|
rɪ̀tá
|
‘hyena’
|
lɪ́tɪ̀
|
‘flute’
|
final
|
bár
|
‘heron’
|
bál
|
‘in
order to’
|
tɔ̀r
|
‘slaughter,
decapitate’
|
tɔ̀l
|
‘be
fat.sg’
|
s : z
|
initial
|
sà
|
‘very,
very much’
|
zà
|
‘dig’
|
sɛ̀s
|
‘delineate,
mark on ground’
|
zɛ̀z
|
‘set
in sights, measure’
|
intervocalic
|
bàsá
|
‘be
good’
|
màzá
|
‘be
tired’
|
final
|
sɛ̀s
|
‘delineate,
mark on ground’
|
zɛ̀z
|
‘set
in sights, measure’
|
s : sʼ
|
initial
|
sɪ̄
|
‘bowl,
pot’
|
sʼɪ́
|
‘diarrhea’
|
sās
|
‘greet’
|
sʼàsʼ
|
‘chest’
|
sɛ̀s
|
‘delineate,
mark on ground’
|
sʼɛ̀sʼ
|
‘termite’
|
final
|
kɪ̄s
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
kɪ́sʼ
|
‘light
fire’
|
hās
|
‘wash
(sth.)’
|
hásʼ
|
‘ruminate,
chew cud’
|
s : ʃ
|
initial
|
sɪ̄
|
‘bowl,
pot’
|
ʃɪ̀
|
‘rain
(v.)’
|
sá
|
‘tree’
|
ʃá
|
‘eat.sg
(soft food)’
|
in CC sequence
|
ɪ́skɪ́r
|
‘squirrel
(sp.)’
|
túʃkír
|
‘heart,
internal organ’
|
final
|
yìs
|
‘circle,
encircle’
|
yìʃ
|
‘penis’
|
tūs
|
‘force
feed’
|
tùʃ
|
‘cotton,
thread’
|
sʼ : z
|
initial
|
sʼɪ́
|
‘be
black’
|
zɪ̀
|
‘be
green’
|
sʼʊ́
|
‘river’
|
zù
|
‘pour’
|
intervocalic
|
bāsʼʊ́
|
‘snake
(sp.)’
|
yàzʊ́
|
‘friend’
|
final
|
gwàsʼ
|
‘buffalo’
|
gwàz
|
‘men’
|
wásʼ
|
‘bite’
|
wʊ̀z
|
‘fish
(sp.)’
|
sʼ : ʃ
|
initial
|
sʼɛ̀
|
‘shuck’
|
ʃɛ̀
|
‘tooth’
|
sʼǎk
|
‘burn’
|
ʃàk
|
‘catch
fish (trad.)’
|
intervocalic
|
wàsʼāgɪ́
|
‘bite,
pain’
|
wàʃɪ́kāgɪ́
|
‘boil’
|
final
|
dɔ̀sʼ
|
‘suck’
|
dɔ̀ʃ
|
‘stand.sg’
|
wàsʼ
|
‘fish’
|
wàʃ
|
‘wife’
|
k : g
|
initial
|
kíʃ
|
‘cocoyam’
|
gìʃ
|
‘hole’
|
kàm
|
‘brother’
|
gàm
|
‘find,
meet’
|
kʊ́sʼ
|
‘be
dry, infertile’
|
gʊ̀sʼ
|
‘swallow’
|
intervocalic
|
ɓākān
|
‘sister’
|
bágà
|
‘jerrycan’
|
kákā
|
‘thorn’
|
kágá
|
‘cackle’
|
final
|
ʃɔ̄k
|
‘move
away, migrate’
|
ʃɔ̀g
|
‘leg,
foot’
|
ták
|
‘overtake,
surpass’
|
tàg
|
‘forehead’
|
k : kʼ
|
initial
|
kɪ̄
|
‘horn
(of animal)’
|
kʼɪ́
|
‘remove
shell’
|
kàm
|
‘brother’
|
kʼàm
|
‘feel’
|
final
|
sʼík
|
‘rat,
mouse’
|
sʼɪ̄kʼ
|
‘be
sour’
|
ʃúk
|
‘land,
alight’
|
ʃʊ̄nkʼ
|
‘vein,
tendon’
|
k : h
|
initial
|
kàm
|
‘brother’
|
hàm
|
‘yawn.pl’
|
kàʊ̄
|
‘bring’
|
hàʊ̄
|
‘come.sg’
|
g : kʼ
|
initial
|
gɪ̄
|
‘with’
|
kɪ̄
|
‘horn
(of animal)’
|
gàm
|
‘find,
meet’
|
kʼàm
|
‘feel’
|
final
|
sɪ̀g
|
‘listen,
hear’
|
sʼɪ̄kʼ
|
‘be
sour’
|
ʃʊ̀g
|
‘palm
tree (sp.)’
|
ʃʊ̄nkʼ
|
‘vein,
tendon’
|
Appendix B: Evidence
for tone contrast: (near) minimal pairs
monosyllabic
|
L
|
|
H
|
|
ày
|
2sg
|
áy
|
‘remain,
stay’
|
sʼɔ̀sʼ
|
‘be
few, small.pl’
|
sʼɔ́sʼ
|
‘scratch’
|
twày
|
‘not
sleep’
|
twáy
|
‘hunger’
|
bʊ̀m
|
‘sugarcane’
|
bʊ́m
|
3pl.poss
|
sà
|
‘very’
|
sá
|
‘tree’
|
|
|
|
|
L
|
|
M
|
|
sʼɔ̀sʼ
|
‘be
few, small.pl’
|
sʼɔ̄sʼ
|
‘be
few, small.sg’
|
bà
|
‘be
wide’
|
bā
|
dem / poss
|
dìʃ
|
‘mushroom
(sp.)’
|
dīʃ
|
‘three’
|
yàʔ
|
‘vomit.sg’
|
yāʔ
|
‘vomit.pl’
|
|
|
|
|
M
|
|
H
|
|
bāl
|
‘beehive’
|
bál
|
‘in
order to’
|
wʊ̄l
|
‘together’
|
wʊ́l
|
‘miss
mark’
|
|
|
|
|
F
|
|
M
|
|
ɓî
|
‘eye’
|
ɓɪ̄
|
‘mountian’
|
|
|
|
|
R
|
|
H
|
|
kɪ̌l
|
‘cattle
egret’
|
kɪ́l
|
‘star’
|
|
|
|
|
R
|
|
M
|
|
ʃǐt
|
‘tree
(sp.)’
|
ʃīt
|
‘wrinkle
(on skin)’
|
|
|
|
|
R
|
|
L
|
|
bǎ
|
‘father’
|
bà
|
‘wake’
|
zǐ
|
‘excrement’
|
zì
|
‘be
green.sg’
|
|
|
|
|
disyllabic
|
L.H
|
|
L.L
|
|
zàmá
|
‘walk
gracefully’
|
zàmà
|
‘wound’
|
|
|
|
|
L.M
|
|
M.H
|
|
mɛ̀ʔī
|
‘food’
|
mɛ̄ʔí
|
‘goats’
|
|
|
|
|
M.L
|
|
M.H
|
|
mātà
|
‘perhaps’
|
mātá
|
‘hunt’
|
|
|
|
|
M.H
|
|
L.H
|
|
ʃɛ̄ʔí
|
‘kale,
collard greens’
|
ʃɛ̀ʔí
|
‘teeth’
|
|
|
|
|
H.M
|
|
L.H
|
|
kákā
|
‘thorn’
|
kàká
|
‘grandmother’
|