A Phonology of Ganza (Gwàmì Nánà)
Joshua
Smolders
SIL International
Ganza
is a previously undescribed Omotic language of the Mao subgroup, and is the
only Omotic language found primarily outside of Ethiopia. This paper presents
the results of nearly a year of phonological fieldwork on Ganza in the form of
a descriptive phonology. Included are presentations of the consonant and vowel
phonemes, syllable structure and phonotactics, notable morphophonemic
processes, and an overview of the tone system. Some interesting features of the
phonology highlighted in this paper include the existence of a nasalizing
glottal stop phoneme, lack of phonemic vowel length, a lexically determined vocalic
alternation between ja~e, and the existence of "construct melodies"
in the tone system. Given that both Omotic languages in general and especially
the Mao subfamily are understudied, this paper provides much-needed data and
analysis for the furtherance of Omotic linguistics.
In spite of over forty years of research in
Omotic languages and numerous calls for descriptive papers and reference
grammars (Hayward 2009:85) to date not a single
descriptive paper has ever been published on the Ganza language. Over the past
decade many other previously under-described Omotic languages have been
covered, such as Dizin (Beachy 2005), Dime (Mulugeta 2008), Sheko (Hellenthal
2010), and Bambassi Mao (Ahland 2012). Several others were known to be in
process during the composition of this paper, including Ganza's closest
relations Hoozo (Getachew 2015) and Sezo (Girma 2015). Ganza, nevertheless,
remains functionally undescribed (see §1.2 for previous research).
In this paper I present an initial phonology
of Ganza, including a description and analysis of consonant and vowel phonemes,
word structure, notable morphophonemic processes, and the tone system. The
paper is structured as follows: In the remainder of this introduction I detail the
methods, persons, and organizations involved in this research (§1.1-§1.2), then
discuss the small body of previous research (§1.3), the genetic classification
of the language (§1.4), and the most current sociolinguistic, geographic, and
demographic information available. In §2 I give a description of and evidence
for the consonant phonemes (§2.1-§2.5) and then discuss the status of consonant
length (§2.6). Likewise for the vowels I describe the phonemes (§3.1) and discuss
a certain case of vocalic free variation (§3.2), the status of length (§3.3), diphthongs
(§3.4), and the limited attestation of vowel assimilation (§3.5). Next I
describe the shape of the word in terms of syllable structure (§4.1) and
phonotactics (§4.2). In §5 I highlight two morphophonemic phenomena of
particular interest, namely the stem allomorphy of a subset of nouns and verbs
via final-vowel elision and vocalic alternation (§5.1), and the interaction of
final-vowel elision, voice and manner assimilation, and final devoicing in
suffixes and clitics (§5.2). Finally, I briefly cover the tonal system,
describing the phonemic tone levels (§6.1), prominent tonal phenomena (§6.2),
and the basic tone melodies attested on nouns (§6.3) and verbs (§6.4). Abbreviations
and references are given after the conclusion in (§7).
All language data used for this paper was
collected in Ethiopia by myself between June 2014 and June 2015. Most of the
data was collected during four two-week sessions in Addis Ababa and one
two-week session in Assosa, the rest being collected on a two-day trip to the
Ganza villages in the Yabeldigis and Penshuba municipal districts.
My primary language informants were Abdurman
Bitu and Mengistu Abdulahi, two mother-tongue Ganza speakers from the municipal
dstrict of Yabeldigis. Both men are also able to speak Gwama [kmq], some Oromo
[orm], and limited Amharic [amh]. The initial language session was conducted
with a translator who spoke with the Ganza in Gwama and with myself in Amharic
and English. After this it was determined that the informants' Amharic and my
growing capacity in Ganza would be sufficient for the remaining sessions. Other
speakers whom I consulted infrequently included Siwar Bitu, Aya Buna, Hawa
Yelke, and Simbara Biya, all from Yabeldigis, as well as Pidan and Dergu (father
names unknown) from Penshuba.
All recordings were made using a USBpre
microphone and processed with Audacity software and feature the voice of
Abdurman Bitu. All acoustic analyzes were done with SIL Speech Analyzer software.
This research was made possible by partnership with the Canada
Institute of Linguistics, Addis Ababa University, and SIL Ethiopia, to whom I
give my deepest thanks. I would like to thank the paper reviewers, whose
comments and corrections helped significantly improve this paper, and my
colleagues in the SIL Ethiopia linguistics department for their continuous
feedback on my analysis. I would like to acknowledge my colleagues at SIL
Ethiopia who assisted with the administrative aspects of the aforementioned
research sessions, and Ramadan Haaron, a native Gwama speaker from Tongo who
assisted as an interpreter for the first session. I also thank the Benishangul-Gumuz
Regional State Ministry of Culture and Tourism and the officials in the
Mao-Komo Special woreda
(administrative district) for their co-opration during my visit to the Ganza
villages. Finally I would like to thank my primary Ganza informants, Abdurman
Bitu and Mangistu Abdulahi, for their patience, hard work, and amiability
throughout the duration of the project, and the rest of the Ganza speakers
involved for their cheerful participation.
In a 2007 survey proposal written
by SIL Ethiopia, previous research on Ganza is described as almost entirely "coincidental
to studies of other languages" (Magnusson et al 2007:1). This absence of original research presents a unique
opportunity for documentation and analysis, which is the ambition of this paper.
To my knowledge, the following is an exhaustive account of the published and
unpublished works produced on the language:
A discussion of the classification and
distribution of the Mao and Komo languages, including Ganza, was published
recently by Küspert (2015). Prior to this a sociolinguistic survey of the
Ganza, Komo, and "Baruun be Magtole" was conducted by SIL which
included a wordlist of approximately two-hundred tokens (Krell 2011). Several
other wordlists also exist, including an unpublished wordlist of around one hundred
tokens and a short phonological and morphological description by Reidhead (1947),
a wordlist of fifty tokens by James (1965), and an unpublished and unanalyzed
African Comparative Word List of approximately seventeen-hundred words collected
by David Ford (2013) from SIL Ethiopia (which I used with permission as a springboard
for elicitation of my own data). An older work done by Burns in 1950 (as cited
in Magnusson et al 2007) contained a
language map which included the Ganza. Ganza is also mentioned in several
broader works on Omotic language (see Hayward 2003 and Bender 2003). Finally,
and perhaps most surprisingly, in the course of my research I encountered an
unpublished manuscript entitled A Ganza
Language Learning Manual by Loriann Hofmeister, a former SIM worker in
Sudan (2010).
Though not a technical linguistic manuscript, this document contained some
important glimpses into the syntax and morphology of the language as well as providing
some basic vocabulary and a few texts. In addition, after contacting Hofmeister,
I was able to obtain several other unpublished documents including demographic
reports (2009), an excel sheet dictionary of approximately five-hundred items (also
used as a basis for elicitation of my own data), and several other texts and
recordings. From my research it appears that there are several dialectcal
differences between the Reidhead and Hofmeister varieties of Ganza and that of
my informants from Ethiopia.
Ganza (ISO 639-3 [gza]) is a Mao language
of the Omotic family found in western Ethiopia and south-eastern Sudan (not to
be confused with eastern South Sudan). According to the Ethnologue, its full
classification is Afro-Asiatic, Omotic,
North, Mao, West, Ganza (Lewis 2014), though I suspect along with Bender (2000:180)
and Ahland (personal communication) that further inquiry will result in its reclassifiction
as a sister language of Bambassi. The remainder of this section is a summary of
the history of the Omotic family and Mao sub-family.
Figure 1 – Mao classification, adapted from Lewis (2014).
Omotic has been described as
the "weakest" or "most divergent" language family of the
Afro-Asiatic phylum (Fleming 1976:299) and for the past fifty years it has also
remained one of the most under-studied (Bender 2000:1). Originally called "West
Cushitic" and classified as a branch of Cushitic, Fleming (1969, 1974) proposed
that it be reorganized as a primary branch of Afro-Asiatic called
"Omotic", a name chosen in reference to the Omo river valley (Bender
1975:39).
More recently scholars such as Thiel (2006, 2012) have questioned whether
Omotic can even rightly be classified as Afro-Asiatic, maintaining that to date
"no convincing arguments have been presented in favour of this Afroasiatic Affiliation Hypothesis"
(2012:369).
Within Omotic the Mao sub-family
(Bender's O8) is the most "data deficient" (Bender 2000:179, 221) and
"undocumented" (Bender 2003:266) of all the sub-families, thus making
its classification within Omotic problematic. In Figure 1, Mao is shown under
"North Omotic" parallel to the Dizoid and Gonga-Gimojan sub-families in
accordance with the Ethnologue's classification (Lewis 2014). In both Bender (2003:1)
and Hayward (2003:242), however, it is proposed as a primary branch of Omotic (see
Figure 2 and Figure 3 below). It is not my intention in this paper to support any
particular one of these classifications, only to address the lack of
documentation which causes difficulty in classification.
Figure 2 - Mao
classification, adapted from Bender (2003:1).
Figure 3 - Mao classification, adapted from Hayward (2003:242). Ganza is not
mentioned in this work.
The name "Mao" bears
some historical baggage. Originally it was used to refer to the language now
known as Anfillo [myo], then classified as a Koman language of the Nilo-Saharan
phylum but now determined to be an Omotic language of the Kefoid sub-group (Bender
2000:179). The name is now most commonly used in reference to the sub-group of
Omotic containing Hoozo, Sezo, Bambassi, and Ganza, as in Hayward and Bender
above. This seems to be an appropriate usage given that in Sezo mawa means 'peoples' (Bender 2000:179), in Hoozo móó means 'person' (Getachew 2015:2), and in Bambassi màw is used as an unparsable autonym
for all four languages of the group— namely Bambassi /màw-és ꜜaːtsʼ-è/ 'Mao language', Hoozo and Sezo /bègí màw-és aːtsʼ-è/ 'Mao language of Begi', and Ganza /sówès
màw-es aːtsʼ-è/ 'Mao Language of Sowes' (Ahland 2012:7). While I have not found
a definite cognate of mawa or màw in Ganza, there is an unparsable
element meaning something like 'person' found in the words /màláꜜ/ 'toddler',
/màmàꜛ/ 'child', /màlíꜜ/ 'son', and /màkíꜜ/ 'daughter'.
The Ganza are a small yet linguistically viable language group. This
is to say that the language is being transmitted to the next generation and the
people are generally proud of their linguistic and cultural heritage (Krell
2011:14). The Ethnologue lists their endangerment level as "6a",
meaning the language is used vigorously in all generations but remains
unstandardized (Lewis 2014). The Ganza's autonym is /ɡwàmìꜛ/ and they call their
language /ɡwàmìꜛ nánà/ meaning 'mouth of the Gwami'. According to the
people themselves the term "Ganza" is a name given to them by Arabic
speakers of Sudan (Krell 2011:10). Although the Ganza live in
an Oromo speaking area of Ethiopia and are adjacent to the Berta, Gwama, Komo,
and Uduk language groups (see Figure 4
below), most Ganza consider themselves to be functionally monolingual (Krell 2011:13).
According to my experience and that of Hofmeister (2009) this holds true, with
only a select number of Ganza speakers utilizing a trade language such as Gwama,
Sudanese Arabic, Uduk, Komo, Afan Oromo, or very rarely Amharic.
Figure 4 – The geo-political and linguistic environment of the Ganza.
The Ganza are presently a
cross-border group, dwelling in both the Blue Nile region of Sudan (Kurmuk
District, between the Yabus and Daga rivers) and in the extreme western parts
of the Benishangul-Gumuz region of Ethiopia. They are the only Omotic language
whose primary population is found outside of Ethiopia. On the Sudan side, they
inhabit the villages of Damo ( Dahmoh), Gondollo, Cape, Hilla Jadid, Bogida,
Dash, Bulu Bulu, Belatuma (not to be confused with the Uduk village of the same
name), Labatz, Musa Ollo, Lakai, Gwasha, Papan, Duga Belle (or Tugubele), Tukul
Ha'a, Mushura, Darsuma (Darsoma), Namu, Hilla Ful, and Boto Ka'a (Hofmeister
2009). In addition to these Krell reports two villages by the names of Korbum
and Yeshkab (Krell 2011:10). On the Ethiopian side they are reported to live in
Yamasala (Krell 2011:10) but when I visited the Mao-Komo Special woreda the Ganza there reported that in
Ethiopia they are only found in Yabeldigis and Penshuba.
Although the precise population
of the Ganza is still unknown, it seems that they are much more numerous than
previously thought. Recent estimates based on linguistic surveys have ranged
between three-thousand (Krell 2011) and five-thousand four-hundred (Jordan et al 2004), with around four-hundred of
these living on the Ethiopia side and the balance in Sudan. These numbers are a
far cry from previous estimates of "150-170 strong men" and
"nearly extinct" (Bender 2000:179). This range is confirmed by a
synthesis of Krell's, Hofmeister's, and my own data. Krell reported that in the
village of Doma alone there were approximately five-hundred twenty-two people (Krell
2011:11). However, Krell only seemed to be aware of five Sudanese villages and
one Ethiopian. Hofmeister, on the other hand, has verbal reports from Ganza
speakers in Sudan who compared twenty other villages to the village of Gondollo
(Hofmeister 2009). Neither Hofmeister nor Krell mention the two villages where
my informants live, Penshuba and Yabeldigis, which from my investigation have
populations of sixty to one-hundred twenty and two-hundred to three-hundred respectively.
Thus, by my calculation in Figure 5 below,
the Ganza have a minimum population of two-thousand six-hundred ten and are
potentially as numerous as five-thousand two-hundred twenty. The total
Ethiopian population, however, remains very small at only two-hundred sixy to
four-hundred twenty people.
|
Villages
|
Relative Size Reported to
Hofmeister
|
Estimated population per
village
|
# of Villages
|
Total
|
Sudanese Population
|
Damo
|
> Gondollo
|
500+
|
1
|
500
|
Gondollo, Cape, Hilla Jadid
|
= Gondollo
|
300~400
|
3
|
900~1,200
|
Bogida, Dash, Bulu Bulu,
Belatuma, Labatz, Musa Ollo, Lakai,
|
< Gondollo "few"
|
50~100
|
7
|
350~700
|
Gwasha, Papan, Duga Belle,
Tukul Ha'a, Mushura, Darsuma, Namu, Hilla Ful, Boto Ka'a, Korbum, Yeshkab,
|
unknown
|
50~200
|
12
|
600~2,400
|
Ethiopian Population
|
Yabeldigis
|
-
|
200~300
|
1
|
200~300
|
Penshuba
|
-
|
60~120
|
1
|
60~120
|
Total estimated population
of Sudan and Ethiopia:
|
2,610~5,220
|
Figure 5 – Estimated population of the Ganza based on synthesised data from
Hofmeister (2009), Krell (2011), and my own data.
In this section of the paper I
present lexical, acoustic, and morphophonemic evidence for the consonant
phonemes of Ganza and their allophones. Note that all phonemic data is demarcated
by /slashes/, whether in the paper body or examples, but that phonetic data is
demarcated by [brackets] in the body only. Thus the non-demarcated data in the
examples should be understood as phonetic. Figures follow these same conventions
unless indicated otherwise (as with the phoneme charts found in Figure 6 and Figure 17).
Ganza has an inventory of twenty-three phonemic consonants, and like
many Omotic languages it maintains a three way contrast in its oral stops and sibilants
between voiceless, ejective, and voiced features. It also follows the areal
pattern of many Omotic and Nilo-Saharan languages of southern Ethiopia in that, while having a modest set of four ejectives, no
pharyngeals or uvular stops are found (Fleming 1976:307). Figure 6 below is a chart of these phonemic consonants,
excluding allophones.
|
bilabial
|
alveolar
|
palato-alveolar
|
palatal
|
velar
|
glottal
|
stops
|
voiceless
|
p
|
t
|
|
|
k
|
|
ejective
|
pʼ
|
tʼ
|
|
|
kʼ
|
|
voiced
|
b
|
d
|
|
|
ɡ
|
ʔ
|
nasal
|
m
|
n
|
|
|
ŋ
|
ʔ̃
|
fricatives
|
voiceless
|
|
s
|
ʃ
|
|
|
h
|
ejective
|
|
sʼ
|
|
|
|
|
voiced
|
|
z
|
|
|
|
|
Liquids
|
lateral
|
|
l
|
|
|
|
|
trill
|
|
r
|
|
|
|
|
semivowel
|
w
|
|
|
j
|
|
|
Figure 6 – Ganza consonant
phoneme inventory.
In (1) I give evidence for a three way
voiceless-ejective-voiced phonemic contrast in the labial stops.
(1)
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/p/
|
pʰùbá
|
'disease'
|
ʔápʰà
|
'uncle'
|
sépʰ
|
'roof'
|
|
/pʼ/
|
pʼùkʰì
|
'red honey'
|
pʼábí ~ pʼá̰bḭ́
|
'gathering'
|
ʃwàʃàp
|
'tarantula'
|
|
/b/
|
bùbá
|
'male'
|
ʔábà
~ ʔáβà
|
'sun'
|
dòp
|
'lion'
|
The phoneme /p/ is
acoustically distinguishable by either the presence of aspiration or lenition. Word-initially
it is most commonly realized as a strongly aspirated [pʰ]. Word-medially and
finally (and occasionally word-initially) /p/ freely varies between [pʰ~ɸ~f],
as illustrated in (2). As C1 in a consonant cluster, this phonemes
is also realized as an unaspirated [p]. This [pʰ~ɸ~f] correspondence is an
extremely common feature of both Omotic languages and western Ethiopian
languages in general.
(2)
|
Free variation of [ pʰ ~ ɸ ~ f ]
|
|
pʰùbá ~ ɸùbá ~ fùbá
|
'disease'
|
|
ʔápʰà
~ ʔáɸà ~ ʔáfà
|
'uncle'
|
|
ʔápʰ ~ ʔáɸ ~ ʔáf
|
'eye'
|
Figure 7 below gives spectrograms of /p/ in each of the three positions,
illustrating the strong aspirated release in the initial, and fricativization in
the medial and final evidenced by spectrally diffuse aperiodic energy.
Figure 7 - Spectrograms for [pʰáꜜí] 'heavy', [ʔáɸà]
'uncle', and [séɸ] 'roof' respectively.
The ejective and voiced labial
phonemes are harder to distinguish from each other, especially in non-initial
positions where contrast is nearly neutralized. This is true in general of the
ejective-voiced contrast in Ganza, but the labial stops are the most difficult
to distinguish.
In the word-initial position the contrast is easily perceptible, with the /b/
phoneme having substantial prevoicing and the /pʼ/ phoneme having none.
/pʼ/ in the initial position often does not have a strong ejective release,
however, being realized instead as a voiceless unaspirated [p] followed by a
creaky quality on the vowel.
Word-medially, both the /b/
and /pʼ/ phonemes are realized with voicing. While my informants sometimes
produced /pʼ/ with a very slight implosive quality [ɓ], it was not obvious or
consistent enough to rely on as a contrastive feature. Thus, the only substantial
perceptive cues to distinguish the two phonemes in this position are the
presence of very slight fricativization of /b/ and creakiness on the vowel
following /pʼ/. In all other respects they are identical. In word-final
position the ejective /pʼ/ and the voiced /b/ are both realized as the
voiceless unreleased stop [p], which coincides with a wider pattern of final
devoicing and deglottalization in the language. The underlying voiced or
voiceless feature of these consonants is revealed when the nominal marker /-di/
is applied.
Here the final consonant of the noun root becomes the C1 of a
consonant cluster, as in (3). Since /b/ is underlyingly voiced the initial consonant
of the /-di/ suffix remains voiced. However, since /pʼ/ is underlyingly voiceless
the initial consonant of the /-di/ suffix assimilates, resulting in the
consonant cluster [pt] (see also §5.2).
(3)
|
|
Word-final devoicing / deglottalization
|
C1 in a consonant cluster ( noun + /-di/
)
|
|
/b/
|
dôp
|
'lion'
|
dóbdì
|
'the/a lion'
|
|
/pʼ/
|
ʃwàʃàp
|
'tarantula'
|
ʃwàʃàptí
|
'the/a tarantula'
|
Figure 8 below gives spectrograms of /b/ in each of the three positions,
illustrating the substantial prevoicing in the initial, voicing in the medial, and
devoicing in the final. Compare them with Figure 9 which gives spectrograms of /pʼ/ in the same positions, showing a
clear lack of prevoicing or aspiration in the initial, voicing in the medial,
and deglottalization in the final. The presence of creaky quality in the vowels
is not visible on these spectrograms.
Figure 8–Spectrograms for [béè ] 'yellow billed kite', [ tʼàβî ] 'oil', and [ dôp ] 'lion'
respectively.
Figure 9 – Spectrograms for [ pʼàlì ] 'girl', [ pʼá̰bḭ́ ]
'gathering', and [ ʃwàʃàp ]
'tarantula' respectively.
The alveolar oral stops behave
much like the labial stops, only the contrast between non-initial ejective /tʼ/
and voiced /d/ is more obvious and there is little if any lenition of the
voiceless /t/. In (4) I give evidence for a three way phonemic contrast in the
alveolar stops.
(4)
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/t/
|
tʰókʰó
|
'foot'
|
ʔátʰá
|
'breast'
|
bàt ~ bàtːʰ
|
'goose'
|
/tʼ/
|
tʼóɗó
|
'black'
|
pʰádà ~ pʰáɗà̰
|
'deer'
|
ʃêt
|
'buffalo'
|
/d/
|
dòkʰò
|
'friend'
|
kʰúꜜdá ~
kʰúꜜɾá
|
'thatch'
|
kʰìkʰìmít
|
'ground hornbill'
|
The phoneme /t/ is realized
with distinct aspiration and a clear lack of voicing in all positions. In the
final position it is sometimes realized as an unreleased voiceless stop, making
it indistinguishable from /tʼ/ or /d/, but unlike these a given token will
freely vary between the unreleased [t] and a long aspirated [tːʰ]. In Figure
10 below I give spectrograms of this phoneme in the three positions. As can be
seen, the characteristic features of the initial and medial realizations are
voicelessness and a high energy release, and in the final position a
significantly longer consonant with final aspiration.
Figure 10 – Spectrograms
for [ tʰókʰó ] 'foot', [ ʔátʰá ] 'breast', and [ bàtːʰ ] 'duck' respectively.
Like the labial phonemes, in
the initial position /tʼ/ and /d/ are distinct from each other by the presence
of prevoicing in the latter.[13]
Furthermore, /tʼ/ is distinguishable
from /t/ by the intensity of its release, which shows significantly less
aperiodic energy, and also by the creaky quality present on the following
vowels. Also like the labials, /tʼ/ and /d/ are both realized as a voiceless
unreleased [t] in the final position, but when placed in a consonant cluster they
display different behaviour, as shown with the addition of the nominal marker /-di/
in (5).
Notice that adjacent to /tʼ/ the suffix /-di/ also fully assimilates in manner
(see also §2.6 examples (25) and (26), and §5.2 examples (48) and (49)).
(5)
|
|
Word-final devoicing / deglottalization
|
C1 in a consonant cluster ( noun + /-di/
)
|
|
/d/
|
kʰìkʰìmít
|
'ground hornbill'
|
kʰìkʰìmíddì
|
'the/a ground hornbill'
|
|
/tʼ/
|
ʃêt
|
'buffalo'
|
ʃéttʼì
|
'the/a buffalo'
|
In the medial position, /d/
and /tʼ/ both have voicing features. They are distinguished however in that /tʼ/
is realized like a weak implosive [ɗ] or preglottalized [ˀd], whereas /d/ is
rhoticized, often being realized as the alveolar tap [ɾ]. In Figure 11 and Figure 12 I give spectrograms for these two phonemes in the three positions. Notice
especially in comparing [pʰáɗà]
and [kʰúꜜdá] that the former shows
no sonorant energy except for the voicing visible in the fundamental frequency,
whereas the latter shows weak sonorant energy across the spectrum as well as a
sharp fall in the fourth formant (F4), a strong characteristic of rhotics.
Figure 11 – Spectrograms for [ tʼóɗó ] 'black', [ pʰáɗà̰ ]
'deer', and [ ʃêt ]
'buffalo' respectively.
Figure 12 - Spectrograms
for [ dòkʰò ] 'friend', [
kʰúꜜɾá ] 'thatch', and [ hàdìt]
'metal' respectively.
Continuing on to velar stops,
in (6) I give evidence for a three way phonemic contrast. In (7), I again show
the differentiation of word-final /kʼ/ and /ɡ/ by their differing behaviour in
a consonant cluster, where C2 assimilates the voicing feature of C1.
(6)
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/k/
|
kʰàbû
|
'bird'
|
kʰákʰí
|
'white'
|
hàwèkʰ
|
'flock'
|
/kʼ/
|
kʼáɡà
|
'cheek'
|
kʼáɡáʃ
~ kʼáɠá̰ʃ
|
'porcupine'
|
màk
|
'fox'
|
/ɡ/
|
ɡáŋá
|
'donkey'
|
kwàɡá
|
'pumpkin'
|
ôk
|
'hat'
|
(7)
|
|
Word-final devoicing / deglottalization
|
C1 in a consonant cluster ( noun + /-di/ )
|
|
/kʼ/
|
màk
|
'fox'
|
màktí
|
'the/a
fox'
|
/ɡ/
|
ôk
|
'hat'
|
óɡdì
|
'the/a hat'
|
As expected these phonemes
display very similar patterns to the alveolars and labials. There is double release
burst of the /k/ phoneme in the initial position, and either strong aspiration
or slight fricativization in the medial and final positions. These characteristic
can be seen in the spectrograms given in Figure 13. With the /kʼ/ phoneme there is intervocalic
voicing and slight implosiveness, as seen in figure Figure 14, as well as a corresponding creaky quality
on a following vowel. Finally with the /ɡ/ phoneme there is significantly more
sonorant energy in the upper formants intervocalically and devoicing word-finally,
as seen in Figure 15.
Figure 13 - Spectrograms for [ kʰàbû ] 'bird', [ kʰákʰí ]
'white', and [ hàwèkʰ]
'flock' respectively.
Figure 14 - Spectrograms
for [ kʼáɡà ] 'cheek', [ kʼáɠáʃ ] 'porcupine', and [ màk ] 'fox' respectively.
Figure 15 - Spectrograms
for [ ɡáŋá ] 'donkey', [ kwàɡâ ] 'pumpkin', and [ ôk ] 'hat'
respectively.
2.2 Nasal
Stops
In Ganza the nasal stop phonemes are fairly
straightforward, with corresponding phonemes to each place of articulation attested
in the oral stops (including glottal, as will be discussed in §2.5). In (8) I
give evidence of a three way contrast among the nasal stops.
(8)
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/m/
|
màmà
|
'child'
|
wáꜜmá
|
'river'
|
ɡìrîm
|
'dim'
|
/n/
|
náꜜná
|
'word'
|
kʰáꜜná
|
|
mìsɡìrín
|
'sand grouse'
|
/ŋ/
|
unattested
|
|
wàŋà
|
'chicken'
|
kʼwàrìŋ
|
'drawing'
|
There are two points here that
need to be addressed, namely the evidence that [ŋ] is a separate phoneme /ŋ/ versus
a coalescence of /nɡ/, and conversely the evidence that [ɲ~ɲj] is a consonant
cluster /nj/ versus a single phoneme /ɲ/.
Regarding the status of /ŋ/, I
argue first that there is systematic justification for defining it as a
separate phoneme. In the oral stops there is a three way place of articulation
contrast, and it is natural that the nasal stops would match this pattern.
While it is true that a lack of this phone in the word-initial position may be
a general indication that it has a different phonemic status than the other
nasal stops, this distribution pattern is shared by two of Ganza's closest
relatives, Bambassi Mao (Ahland 2012) and Sezo (Girma 2015). Second, there is
lexical evidence that /ŋ/ as a single consonant contrasts with the NC clusters
/ŋkʼ/ and /ŋɡ/, as shown in (9). There is therefore little plausibility for
suggesting [ŋ] is a coalescence of /n/ plus a velar consonant.
(9)
|
|
Evidence of contrast between /ŋ/ - /ŋkʼ/ - /ŋɡ/.
|
|
/ŋ/
|
ʔàŋà |
'sorghum'
|
síŋô
|
'cloud'
|
/ŋkʼ/
|
ʔíŋꜜkʼá
|
'to do'
|
kʼáŋkʼó
|
́'spoon'
|
/ŋɡ/
|
ʔìŋɡì
|
'this (feminine)'
|
sáŋɡô
|
'stringed
instrument'
|
Regarding the status of the
cluster /nj/, the same arguments apply to the opposite effect. First, there is
no systematic justification for a palatal nasal /ɲ/ given a complete lack
of non-sonorant palatal consonants
(excepting the palato-alveolar /ʃ/, which will be discussed in §2.3). Second,
/nj/ has extremely limited distribution and frequency, being attested in only
three of the over one-thousand items in my data corpus. These are given in (10).
Further, two of these three words are likely onomatopoeic forms, representing a
bird call and a cat cry.
(10)
|
All attested occurrences of /nj/
|
|
ʔíꜜɲjá ~ ʔíꜜɲá
|
'to refuse' ,
|
|
ɲjákʰ ~ ɲákʰ
|
|
ɲjàú ~ ɲàú
|
'cat'
|
For cross-linguistic comparison
I have included Figure 16 below showing
the attestation of the nasal phonemes in Ganza's linguistic neighbours. As can
be seen, among the Omotic group /ɲ/ is never attested as a phoneme and /ŋ/ is
attested in two of the three other Mao languages, giving strong typological
justification for my analysis. Among the non-Omotic groups, both phonemes are
attested but /ŋ/ is by far the more common.
|
Language
|
/m/
|
/n/
|
/ŋ/
|
/ɲ/
|
Source
|
|
|
|
|
|
-
|
Ahland
2012
|
Hoozo
[hzo]
|
|
✓
|
-
|
-
|
Getachew
2015
|
Sezo
[sze]
|
✓
|
✓
|
✓
|
|
|
Borna ("Shinasha") [bwo]
|
✓
|
✓
|
-
|
|
|
Anfillo [myo]
|
✓
|
✓
|
-
|
|
|
Shekkacho ("Moča") [moy]
|
✓
|
✓
|
-
|
|
|
Non-Omotic
|
Gwama
[kmq]
|
✓
|
✓
|
✓
|
-a
|
|
Komo
[xom]
|
✓
|
✓
|
✓
|
-
|
Teshome
2006
|
Oromo
[orm]
|
✓
|
✓
|
-
|
✓
|
|
Bertha
[wti]
|
✓
|
✓
|
✓
|
- b
|
|
Gumuz
[guk]
|
✓
|
✓
|
✓
|
✓c
|
Ahland
2004
|
|
|
Figure 16 – Attestation of nasal stops as fully realized
phonemes in languages geographically proximate to Ganza.
Ganza
has four phonemic sibilants. In (11) I give evidence for a four-way contrast
among the fricatives between voiceless, voiced, ejective, and palatalized
fricatives. The only anomaly in the distribution of these sibilants is the lack
of /z/ word-finally.
(11) |
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/s/
|
sásá
|
'bite'
|
sásô
|
'monkey'
|
kʰís
|
|
/z/
|
zólèŋ
|
|
zázò
|
|
not attested
|
|
/sʼ/
|
sʼálò
|
'worm'
|
sʼásʼà
|
|
kʰùsʼ
|
'flower'
|
|
/ʃ/
|
ʃòʃó
|
|
ʃáʃî
|
|
kʰíʃ
|
'forest'
|
/ʃ/ is likely a historical coalescence of /s/ and /j/ which has
now become an independent phoneme. There are two lines of evidence for this.
First, in certain lexical items speakers manifest free variation between [s~ʃ]
when preceding front vowels, examples of which are given in (12). This suggests
that /s/ has a tendency towards palatalization before front vowels.
(12)
|
Instances of free-variation
between [s~ʃ]
|
|
nìsí~nìʃí
|
'how many?'
|
sísô~ʃísô
|
'within'
|
Second, the presence of an old palatal onglide can be detected
in the allomorphic stem of certain words. As will be discussed further in §5.1,
in many Ganza words there is an allomorphic correspondence between the sequence
/ja/ and the vowel /e/. This correspondence is also true of certain words
containing the sequence /ʃa/, suggesting that at one point the phonemic
sequence was /ʃja/ or /sja/. Compare the examples given in (13).
(13)
|
Allomorphic
correspondence between /ʃa/ and /ʃe/.
|
|
ʃáŋkʼâ à ʃéŋkʼ
|
'rock'
|
cf.
|
kʼjábá à kʼéb
|
'to hear'
|
One final comment needs to be made regarding Hayward's
observation of "Sibilant Harmony", which claims that in Omotic
languages palatal (including palato-alveolar) and non-palatal sibilants do not
co-occur in well-formed roots (Hayward 1986). Besides the tendency for /s/ to
palatalize as mentioned in (12), this holds true for Ganza. Actually, the
generalization can be extended to ejective features as well. Thus while we find
that word roots intermingle /s/ and /z/ freely, /sʼ/ and /ʃ/ do not co-occur
with any other sibilant but themselves.
2.4 Liquids and Semivowels
There
are four phonemes for liquids and semivowels in Ganza: the alveolar lateral
/l/, the alveolar trill /r/, and the palatal and labiovelar semivowels /j/ and
/w/. Of these, the phonemic distinction between [l] and [r] is probably the
hardest to distinguish. In certain lexical items the two sounds appear to be in
free variation word-finally. Also [r] is not attested in the initial position. Thus the clearest contrast
is found word-medially, where there are several near-minimal pairs attested in
my data corpus. This evidence for a phonemic contrast between /l/ and /r/ is
given in (14).
(14)
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/l/
|
lìŋí
|
'ditch'
|
kʼùlá
|
|
ɡúmbìl
|
'robe'
|
|
láꜜɡúláɡù
|
|
màlí
|
|
twáŋꜜɡál
|
|
/r/
|
not attested
|
kʰúrà
|
|
ɡábîr
|
'sheep'
|
|
|
|
|
màrì
|
|
ʔáŋɡâr
|
'bed'
|
In (15) I give evidence of contrast for the semi-vowels /w/ and
/j/. For the present I have analyzed these phonemes as consonants in all
positions, including the coda where they could be alternatively be analyzed as
the V2 of a diphthong (see §3.4).
(15)
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/j/
|
jéʃô
|
'rainy season'
|
wájà
|
'ear'
|
pʰâj
|
'heavy'
|
/w/
|
wíɡì
|
|
ʔáwà
|
|
hâw
|
'go'
|
A very interesting feature of Ganza's
consonant system is found in the glottal consonants. Phonetically speaking
there are only two glottal consonants in Ganza, the stop [ʔ] and the fricative
[h], which contrast word-initially and to a lesser extent word-medially. However,
the word-medial glottal stop can be further subdivided by its behaviour into a
plain glottal stop /ʔ/ and a nasal glottal stop /ʔ̃/.
This nasal glottal phoneme is identified by the presence of unmotivated (lexical)
nasalization on both on the preceding and following vowels.
In (16) I give evidence for phonemic contrast between these three glottal
phonemes.
(16)
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
/ʔ/
|
ʔâw
|
'say'
|
sáʔà
|
'goat'
|
|
/ʔ̃/
|
not
attested
|
|
sã̀ʔĩ̂
|
'bead jewelry'
|
|
/h/
|
hâw
|
|
sáhánà
|
'dish'
|
The glottalic consonants
differ significantly in their distribution patterns from the other consonantal
phonemes: First, none of the three phonemes appear unambiguously word-finally.
The oral stop does appear syllable-finally in allomorphic stems where there is
final-vowel elision, such as in /wáꜜʔá/ à [wéʔ] 'run' (see §5.1).
It also appears word-finally in one lexical item, /lěʔlěʔ/ 'continuously',
which appears to some sort of reduplicated form. There are also a few lexical
items where I suspect the /ʔ̃/ phoneme occurs syllable finally, for example /màʔ̃tʼáꜜ/ 'sugar cane'
which is phonetically realized as [mã̀ɗã̂] and can be contrasted
with non-nasalized /máꜜtʼáʃ/ 'bone', phonetically [máꜜɗáʃ].
Second, /h/ has an extremely limited medial
distribution. It is found in only four lexical items, three of which are likely
borrowings and the fourth having an anomalous nasal feature akin to that found
on the glottal nasal.
/ʔ/ and /ʔ̃/ on
the other hand show a significant number of contrasts, as shown in (17).
(17)
|
Further word-medial attestations of /ʔ/ and /ʔ̃/.
|
sáʔà
|
'goat'
|
sã̀ʔĩ̂
|
'bead jewelry'
|
|
háʔī
|
'death'
|
hã́ʔã̀
|
'water'
|
|
háʔō
|
'war'
|
kʼjã̀ʔã́
|
'egg'
|
|
jóʔó
|
|
nã́ʔĩ̀
|
'daughter'
|
|
kʰáʔà
|
|
mã̀ʔĩ̀
|
'to rip in two'
|
|
nàʔà
|
|
dã̀ʔĩ́
|
'hammer'
|
|
ʔòʔò
|
|
pʼṍʔṍ
|
'Cheleda baboon'
|
|
tʰáʔà
|
|
zĩ̀ʔĩ́~zĩ̀ĩ́
|
'green'
|
|
kʼwáʔàsʼ
|
|
sẽ̀ʔẽ́
|
'to not comply'
|
Third, the primary contrast
between /ʔ/ and /h/ can be found word-initially. Proof for this can be seen when an open-syllable
morpheme is placed to the left of the verb stem. In this environment a word
with an initial /ʔ/ is realized as an intervocalic [ʔ] whereas an initial /h/
is phonetically deleted, resulting in vowel hiatus. In (18) and (19) I give an
example of this using the minimal pair /háw/ 'go' and /ʔáwꜜ/ 'say' set in the
basic affirmative verb clause , which begins with the affirmative particle
/hàꜛ/ plus a subject clitic followed by the verb root and a verbal clitic.,
(18)
|
Intervocalic deletion of /h/
|
|
/háw/
|
'go'
|
→
|
hàɡááwbô
|
|
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
háw=bo /
|
|
|
|
aff=3m.sbj go=vc1
|
|
|
|
|
'he goes'
|
|
Intervocalic preservation of /ʔ/
|
|
/ʔáwꜜ/
|
'say'
|
→
|
hàɡáʔáwbò
|
|
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
ʔáwꜜ=bo /
|
|
|
|
|
aff=3m.sbj say=vc1
|
|
|
|
|
|
It could be argued that this
pattern actually shows contrast between the glottal fricative phoneme /h/ and
an onsetless vowel. The presence of the glottal found in situations such as (19)
could then be attributed to glottalic epenthesis rather than preservation.
However, if this were the case, I would expect both /háw/ and */áwꜜ/ to display
glottal epenthesis intervocalically, since after the deletion of the [h] the
two environments would be identical.
One final thing to mention regarding the
glottal consonants is that in two high-frequency lexical items I observed
alternation between the oral glottal stop [ʔ] and the voiced velar stop [ɡ].
First, in (20a) the third-person subject clitic
/=ɡa/ freely varies between [ɡa~ʔa] in connected speech. Second, as in (20b) I
observed a pronunciation difference between my two speakers in their word for
'thing', the one using [jóʔó] and the other [jóɡó].
(20)
|
a.
|
Free-variation between [ɡ ~ ʔ]
|
b.
|
Lexical alternation between [ɡ ~ ʔ]
|
|
|
hàɡá náꜜkʰábô ~ hàʔá náꜜkʰábô
|
|
jóʔó ~ jóɡó
|
'thing'
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa náꜜká=bo /
|
|
|
|
|
|
aff=3m.sbj
big=vc1
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Having just described the consonantal
phonemic inventory of Ganza it is fitting to discuss the status of consonant
length before moving on to vowel phonemes. The presence of phonemic gemination
in the consonant system is a well-known feature of the Ethiopian Language Area (Crass
& Meyer 2008:231), thus it cannot be ignored in any phonological
description of an Ethiopian language. This is especially true when it is
claimed to be absent, as I do here.
A good example of environment
(a) is the morpheme boundary between the negative suffix /-ánꜜ/ and the verbal clitic /=na/ in
negative verb constructions. This is an interesting situation because the
primary contrastive cue between the negative and positive forms of the verb here
could be analyzed as consonant gemination.
Examples of this are given in (21), (22), and (23).
(21)
|
ʔàsìtká
|
ɡàrànà
|
cf.
|
ʔàsìt
|
ɡàráánnà
|
|
/ʔàsìꜛ-di=ɡa
|
ɡàrà=na/
|
|
/ʔàsìꜛ-di
|
ɡàrá-ánꜜ=na/
|
person-nm=3m.sbj
|
sit.sg=vc2
|
|
person-nm
|
sit.sg-neg=vc2
|
'the person sits'
|
|
'the person does not sit'
|
(22)
|
hàɡá
|
úʃìnà
|
cf.
|
ʔúꜜʃíánnà
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
|
úʃì=na/
|
|
/ʔúʃì-ánꜜ=na/
|
aff=3m.sbj
|
tie=vc2
|
|
tie-neg=vc2
|
'he ties'
|
|
'he does not tie'
|
(23)
|
hàɡá
|
sàsnà
|
cf.
|
sásáánnà
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
|
sàs=na/
|
|
/sásá-ánꜜ=na/
|
aff=3m.sbj
|
old=vc2
|
|
old-neg=vc2
|
'he is old'
|
|
'he is not old'
|
I do not consider consonant
length here to be the main cue of negation for several reasons. First, the
primary contrastive feature appears to be tonal. Not only does the negative
suffix /-ánꜜ/ bear a conspicuous H tone,but the negative mood also causes the verb
root to take its citation tone melody
as opposed to its construct melody
(which is used with /=na/ in the affirmative). For definitions of citation and construct melodies see §6. Second, when /-ánꜜ/ is combined with
other verbal morphology, such as the imperative suffixes in (24), the primary
contrastive cue cannot be consonant length since there is no doubled consonant.
(24)
|
ɡàráʃ
|
cf.
|
ɡàrááꜜnéʃ
|
|
/ɡàrá-éʃ/
|
|
/ɡàrá-ánꜜ-éʃ/
|
sit.sg-imp.sg
|
|
sit.sg-neg-imp.sg
|
'sit!'
|
|
'don't sit!'
|
A good example of environment
(b) is found at the morpheme boundary between a noun root which ends in an
alveolar or palato-alveolar oral consonant and the nominal marker /-di/. Here
the /d/ of the suffix fully assimilates in manner and voicing to the final
consonant in the noun root, forming a long consonant. A contextualized example
of this is given in (25) followed by examples in (26) with each of the oral
alveolar phonemes.
(25)
|
ʔìntʰì
|
ɡábírrì
|
ʔàɡá
|
nákʰànà
|
|
/ʔìntìꜛ
|
ɡábírꜜ-di
|
hàꜛ=ɡa
|
nákà=na/
|
dem.prox
|
sheep-nm
|
aff=3m.sbj
|
big=vc2
|
'this sheep, he is big'
|
(26)
|
|
Isolation
|
Manner assimilation
of /-di/ suffix
|
|
/r/
|
ɡábîr
|
'sheep'
|
ɡábírrì
|
'the/a
sheep'
|
|
/l/
|
jàwǐl
|
'hyena'
|
jàwìllí
|
'the/a
hyena'
|
|
/t/
|
hàdìtʰ
|
'metal'
|
hàdìttʰí
|
'the/a metal'
|
|
/d/
|
kʰìkʰìmít
|
'ground hornbill'
|
kʰìkʰìmíddì
|
'the/a ground hornbill'
|
|
/tʼ/
|
ʃêt
|
'buffalo'
|
ʃéttʼì
|
'the/a
buffalo'
|
|
/s/
|
ɡàmìs
|
'shirt'
|
ɡàmìssí
|
'the/a
shirt'
|
|
/ʃ/
|
kʼóꜜŋóʃ
|
'hunchback'
|
kʼóꜜŋóʃʃí
|
'the/a
hunchback'
|
|
/sʼ/
|
ɡíŋɡìlísʼ
|
'lovebird'
|
ɡíŋɡìlíssʼí
|
'the/a lovebird'
|
Since both these environments
are found at morpheme boundaries it is safe to say that Ganza does not utilize
consonant length phonemically. This is not to say that the length is entirely
unperceived by the native Ganza speaker, but rather that since it is unattested
in monomorphemic words it is non-contrastive in the Ganza phonology.
3.
Vowels
In this section of the paper I describe and
discuss the five vowel phonemes of Ganza. I also address the unpredictable
(lexical) free-variation between [wa~o], the lack of phonemic long vowels, the
phonemic composition of diphthongs, and one small attestation of vowel harmony.
As can be expected of an Omotic language,
Ganza has a system of five phonemic vowels contrasting high and mid
front-unrounded vowels, high and mid back-rounded vowels, and a single low
vowel. Figure 17 is a chart of these
phonemic vowels, excluding allophones.
|
Front
|
Back
|
High
|
i
|
u
|
Mid
|
e
|
o
|
Low
|
a
|
Figure 17 – Ganza vowel
phoneme inventory.
Below I give evidence of
contrast between these vowel phonemes, in (27a) comparing the high front, mid
front, and low vowels /i, e, a/, and in (27b) comparing the high back, mid
back, and low vowels /u, o, a/.
(27)
|
a.
Minimal or near-minimal pairs between / i, e, a /
|
|
|
Word-initial
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/i/
|
ʔínsá
|
'tree'
|
sʼílí
|
'to add'
|
ʔánsʼí
|
'month'
|
|
/e/
|
ʔénsó
|
'request'
|
sʼélé
|
|
jìlànsʼé
|
'wildcat'
|
|
/a/
|
ʔánꜜsʼó
|
'rain'
|
sʼáló
|
'worm'
|
ʔànsʼà
|
'gold'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
b.
Minimal or near-minimal pairs between / u, o, a /
|
|
|
|
Word-medial
|
Word-final
|
|
/u/
|
ʔúlú
|
|
ʃúꜜmú
|
'to be leftover'
|
kʰùɡú
|
'owl'
|
|
/o/
|
ʔólò
|
|
ʃóꜜmó
|
'python'
|
kʰóɡò
|
'hill'
|
|
/a/
|
ʔàlà
|
|
ʃàmá
|
'to be tired'
|
kʰáɡà
|
'possession'
|
For the purposes of acoustic
description Figure 18 below shows the
average and median first formant (F1) and second formant (F2) measurements for
these vowels. For each vowel I measured twenty tokens, ten in which the target vowel
was between consonants and ten in which the vowel was word-final. As much as
possible, tokens were chosen in order to represent a variety of tone and onset
consonants. Measurements were taken from the second repetition of the token in
a given recording, and at approximately one-third of the way into the duration of
the vowel.
|
F1 average
|
F1 median
|
F2 average
|
F2 median
|
/i/
|
355
|
354
|
2092
|
2120
|
/e/
|
484
|
482
|
1896
|
1867
|
/u/
|
380
|
375
|
986
|
962
|
/o/
|
487
|
473
|
941
|
916
|
/a/
|
694
|
689
|
1395
|
1385
|
Figure 18 – Average and median
formant values for Ganza vowels given in hertz (Hz).
When the raw numbers are
plotted on a formant chart, as in Figure
19 below, the classic V of a five-vowel system
is evident, but with one aberration: there is significant overlap in realizations
of /o/ and /u/.
Figure 19 - Vowel formant plot of one-hundred Ganza lexical items.
There
is a curious feature of Ganza's vowel phonology in which free variation exists
between [wa~ʔo] for a certain set of words beginning with /wa/ but is absent in
all other words beginning with /wa/. For the items in (28a), I received both
phonetic variants of the words from both of my language helpers when the words
were elicited in isolation. Compare these with (28b) in which there is no
attested free variation. In addition, when the average F1 and F2 values of the [w]
in the (a) items were measured they turned out to be significantly different from
those of [w] in the (b) items.
(28)
|
a. Items with [wa ~ ʔo]
free variation.
|
b. Items with no
variation.
|
|
wáꜜmá ~ ʔóꜜmá
|
'river'
|
wáꜜsí
|
'meat'
|
|
wáɗà ~ ʔóɗà
|
'insult'
|
wàʃàl
|
'hare'
|
|
wápʰà ~ ʔópʰà
|
|
wáŋà
|
'ensete'
|
|
wájà ~ ʔójà
|
|
wàŋà
|
'chicken'
|
I see two probable explanations for this unpredictable and
asymetrical variation. The first is a diachronic hypothesis in which the proto vowel
inventory did not contain /o/, but rather /o/ developed historically from the
coalescence of /wa/. After /o/ was established as a phoneme, new combinations
of /ʔua/ or /wa/ resulted in the non-varying /wa/ items. The varying /wa/
items, then, would be higher frequency words which preserved the old /wa/ instead
of coalescing. The second hypothesis is synchronic, in which the /w/ semivowel
is actually underlyingly two different vowel onsets. Thus, while non-varying
items such as [wàŋà] 'chicken' begin underlyingly with /ʔua/, varying items
such a [wáꜜmá ~ ʔóꜜmá] 'river' begin underlyingly with /ʔoa/. I do not
attempt here to make an argument for one of these cases over the other.
Analogous to [wa~ʔo], Ganza also has an alternation between [ja~e].
Like the [wa~ʔo] variation, this only occurs in certain lexical items. However,
as will be explained further in §5.1, this is predictable stem allomorphy, not
free variation as with [wa~ʔo].
Like
consonant length, contrastive vowel length is a common feature of Ethiopian
languages. In Omotic languages especially it is expected that the five phonemic
vowels will each have long counterparts. I was thus surprised to discover that
Ganza does not have a clear contrast between long and short vowel phonemes.
Instead, Ganza has predictable utterance-final vowel lengthening and a set of
monosyllabic words with double vowels.
Acoustically speaking, Ganza has three significantly different
phonetic levels of vowel length. The first and shortest level corresponds to
the standard short vowel. The second, slightly lengthened set corresponds to utterance-final
lengthened vowels. The third and longest level corresponds to all monosyllabic open-syllable
free words (i.e. CVV, Cw/jVV). Figure 20 gives
the average and median duration for each vowel phoneme in these three
environments as found in isolated words.
|
|
Utterance-final
|
CVV words
|
average
|
median
|
average
|
median
|
average
|
median
|
/i/
|
126
|
125
|
208
|
196
|
335
|
347
|
/e/
|
140
|
129
|
231
|
227
|
348
|
325
|
/u/
|
106
|
100
|
197
|
216
|
304
|
304
|
/o/
|
124
|
126
|
203
|
194
|
335
|
335
|
/a/
|
120
|
124
|
180
|
180
|
340
|
329
|
Figure 20 – Average and median duration of vowels in three environments,
given in milliseconds (ms).
These numbers are somewhat exaggerated compared to connected
speech. However, comparative measurements of vowel lengths within a single
sentence still attest three phonetic lengths corresponding to these three
environments.
For the purpose of phonemic analysis it is the third level of
length that is most interesting. While the utterance-final lengthening can be
dismissed as a predictable (phonetic) process, the question is whether this very limited
set of CVV words are sufficient grounds for claiming that vowel length is contrastive.
In (29) I list all the words I have found to date that contain these long
vowels.
(29)
|
a.
|
CVV nouns
|
b.
|
CVV verbs
|
c.
|
CVV pronouns
|
|
|
kʼjóò
|
'body'
|
|
kʰáꜜá
|
'to work'
|
|
tʰìì
|
1sg
|
|
|
kʰáà
|
'labour'
|
|
máꜜá
|
'to eat'
|
|
mùù
|
|
|
|
níꜜí
|
'father'
|
|
kʰòó
|
'to carry'
|
|
jéé
|
2sg
|
|
|
náꜜá
|
'mother'
|
|
ʃáá
|
'to see'
|
|
kʰíꜜí
|
|
|
|
bàâ
|
'father'
|
|
kʰwáꜜá
|
'to come'
|
|
kʰúꜜú
|
|
|
|
sáꜜá
|
'woman,
wife'
|
|
tʼóꜜó
|
'to come'
|
|
|
|
|
|
ʔòò
|
'grandmother'
|
|
tʼáꜜá
|
'to drip'
|
|
|
|
|
|
sʼwíꜜí
|
'hip'
|
|
kʼáá
|
'to eat meat'
|
|
|
|
|
|
kʼàà
|
'new
thing'
|
|
kʼàà
|
'to be new'
|
|
|
|
|
|
tʼéꜜé
|
'hornbill'
|
|
kʼéꜜé
|
'to seem'
|
|
|
|
|
|
béè
|
'black
kite'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
pʼwíì
|
'type
of tree'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
síì
|
'wax'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Like long consonants, I analyze these vowels as double vowels,
not long vowels, which have arisen from the historical elision of a consonant.
That is to say, [kʼjóò] 'body' should be analyzed as /kʼjóò/ not /kʼjôː/,
and likely came from a source word */kʼjóCò/. As for the identity of the
elided consonant, I hypothesize that [aa] items came from an elided [h], [oo]
and [uu] from an elided [w], and [ee] and [ii] from an elided [j]. Below are
outlined various evidences which lead me to this conclusion:
a. All monosyllabic free words are heavy syllables.
The attested profiles on monosyllabic words are CVV, CCVV, CVC,
CCVC, CVCC. Since there are no CV noun or verb roots for a CVV word to contrast
with, length contrast is essentially neutralized in monosyllabic free words. It
is likely that originally all noun and verb roots were minimally disyllabic,
and that CVV and CVC stems were formed through historical syncope and apocope
respectively (see §5.1 for evidence of this occurring synchronically).
b. Most CVV words are high frequency tokens.
With a few exceptions, the above words are all extremely high-frequency
tokens. Since high-frequency tokens undergo phonological reduction faster than
other words, they tend to be the locus of such phonological irregularities
(Bybee 2004:113). It is therefore plausible that the elision of a consonant
would occur in Ganza in these high-frequency tokens while not occurring in
other, less frequent items (cf. /tìʔìꜛ/ 'to protect' and /tììꜛ/ '1sg').
c. Most CVV words have a CV allomorph.
In
the verb system, CVV verbs predictably shorten to CV in certain grammatical
frames, essentially displaying the same allomorphic pattern of certain CVʔV
verbs which elide to CVʔ (see §5.1). Likewise, when the independent pronouns
function as possessives they take a short form, as illustrated in (30).
(30)
|
sùdánsà
|
háwssí
|
tʼélꜜbó,
|
tìì
|
cf.
|
tì
|
ɡáŋà
|
|
/Sùdánꜜ-sa
|
háw-sa=di
|
tʼélꜜ=ꜛ=bo,
|
tììꜛ/
|
|
/tìꜛ
|
ɡàŋà/
|
Sudan-dir
|
go-purp=1sg.sbj
|
want=irr=vc1
|
1sg
|
|
1sg.poss
|
donkey
|
'I want to go to Sudan'
|
|
'my
donkey'
|
d. Tonal melodies on monosyllabic noun and verb roots give evidence
that they were originally disyllabic.
If these roots were originally monosyllabic I would expect to
find only /L/ and /H/ citation melodies attested. However, for the CVV verbs
and pronouns there are three attested citation melodies— /HꜜH/, /H/, and /LH/— and
for the CVV nouns there are four— /HꜜH/, /Hꜜ/, /LHꜜ/, and /Lꜛ/. The specifics
of this will be discussed further in §6, but for the moment it is sufficient to
say that these melodies are more complex than would be expected on monosyllabic
roots and that they bear no differences to the melodies attested on disyllabic
roots.
e. [h], [w], and [j] are unattested in the proposed environments of
elision.
There is only one attestation of an intervocalic [h] in
non-borrowed words, /máhì/ 'feline', but this is not between homorganic
vowels and sometimes surfaces with a suspicious nasal feature [mã́hĩ̀]. Also,
as was shown in §2.5 example (18), when [h] is found intervocalically at a
morpheme boundary it deletes. Similarly, there are no attestations of [w] between
two back-rounded vowels or [j] between two front-unrounded in well-formed roots
(i.e. *[owo, uwu, uwo, owu], *[eje, iji, eji, ije]). It is plausible then that
these consonants historically elided in these environments resulting in double
vowels.
f. At least four high-frequency tokens have been observed to form a
double vowel after undergoing stem allomorphy.
When modified, the words /wájà/ 'ear' and /kwájà/ 'place' take
the allomorphic forms /wéè/ and /kwéè/ respectively, as shown in (31) and (32).
Here we actually observe the formation of a double vowel via stem allomorphy
(see §5.1), with the sequence
/aja/ becoming /ee/.
(31)
|
wájà
|
'ear'
|
cf.
|
tʰì wéè
|
'my
ear'
|
(32)
|
kʰwájàtkà
|
náꜜkʰábô
|
cf.
|
ʔìtʰí
|
kʰwéétkà
|
náꜜkʰábô
|
|
/kwájà-di=ɡa
|
náꜜká=bo/
|
|
/ʔìtí
|
kwéè-di=ɡa
|
náꜜká=bo/
|
place-nm=3m.sbj
|
big=vc1
|
|
dem.dist
|
place-nm=3m.sbj
|
big=vc1
|
'the
place is big'
|
|
'that
(far) place is big'
|
Similarly the verbs /háw/ 'go' and /ʔáwꜜ/
'say' take the allomorphic forms /hááꜜ/ and /ʔááꜜ/ in certain grammatical
environments, as in (33), suggesting the proto-roots */hawa/ and */ʔawa/.
(33)
|
hàdí
|
ʔáwnà
|
cf.
|
hàdí
|
ʔááꜜɡwánà
|
|
/hàꜛ=di
|
ʔáwꜜ=na/
|
|
/hàꜛ=di
|
ʔááꜜ-ɡwáꜜ=na/
|
aff=1sg.sbj
|
say=vc2
|
|
aff=1sg.sbj
|
say-pfv=vc2
|
'I
say'
|
|
'I
have said'
|
From these evidences I conclude that instances of long vowel
found in Ganza are doubled vowels rather than phonemically long vowels which
contrast with phonemically short vowels. Further, I hypothesize that these
double vowels have their source in the historical elision of a consonant, most
likely [h] or [ʔ] in the case of [aa], [j] in the case of [ii] and [ee], and [w]
in the case of [uu] and [oo].
Phonetically,
Ganza has only two diphthongs, [au] and [ai]. However, phonemically these are
best analyzed as consonant codas /aw/ and /aj/, counterparts to the onglides
/wa/ and /ja/. Examples of these are given in (34).
(34)
|
Semivowel Onglides
|
Semivowel Codas
|
|
/wa/
|
kʼwáɗà
|
'to swallow'
|
/aw/
|
kʰáꜜú
|
'to
roast'
|
|
|
pʰwáɗá
|
'naked-neck chicken'
|
|
ɲjàú
|
'cat'
|
|
/ja/
|
tʼjáꜜlá
|
'to
want, seek'
|
/aj/
|
pʰáì
|
'to
be heavy'
|
|
|
sʼjáná
|
'dry
season'
|
|
wùʃáì
|
'francolin
(bird)'
|
The main reason for analyzing these as consonant codas is that they
behave as such in terms of syllabification and tone distribution. For example,
when elicited in isolation the verb /ʔáwꜜ/ 'say' is realized as [ʔáù], with the
/HL/ melody surfacing as falling tone spread over the diphthong. When this verb
is elicited in context, however, the L is not realized on the /w/ coda but
instead delinks, either causing downstep as in (35a), or reattaching to a
following toneless morpheme as in (35b). Thus, except when elicited in
isolation these phones do not serve as tone-bearing units (TBU). Also, when the
morpheme following /ʔáwꜜ/ begins with a vowel, the /w/ coda is reassigned as
the onset of that morpheme, as in (36). If this were a true diphthong I would
expect further evidence of vowel hiatus.
(35)
|
a.
|
L delinks from /w/ causing downstep
|
b.
|
L delinks from /w/ and attaches to /=bo/
|
|
|
nótnà
|
ʔáwꜜlé
|
|
hàdí
|
ʔáwbò
|
|
|
/nóꜜ-di=na
|
ʔáwꜜ-lé/
|
|
/hàꜛ=di
|
ʔáwꜜ=bo/
|
|
what-nm=2sg.sbj
|
say-q
|
|
aff=1sg.sbj
|
say=vc1
|
|
|
|
|
(36)
|
/w/ coda is
reassigned as syllable onset when followed by /-ánꜜ/
|
|
tìì
|
ʔáꜜwánnà
|
|
/tììꜜ
|
ʔáwꜜ-ánꜜ=na
/
|
1sg
|
say-neg=vc2
|
|
The strongest
counterevidence to my above conclusion is that when the future suffix /-sa/ and
a subject clitic are added to a verb root ending in a diphthong, the
morphophonemic processes which take place seem to treat that coda like a vowel.
As will be discussed in §5.2, the suffix /-sa/ will elide its final vowel and
merge with the clitic if the verb root to which it attaches ends in an open-syllable.
This is exactly what occurs in (37) where /ʔáwꜜ-sa=ɡa=na/ 'he will say' is
realized as [ʔáwskànà] and not *[ʔáwsàɡànà], suggesting that the
phonology treats the coda of /ʔáwꜜ/ as a vowel.
(37)
|
ʔáwskànà
|
|
/ʔáwꜜ-sa=ɡa=na/
|
say-fut=3m.sbj=vc2
|
'he will say'
|
However, the phonology
does not treat coda of /ʔawꜜ/ like a vowel with the addition of a VC suffix,
such as the imperative marker /-éʃ/. Elsewhere, when this morpheme attaches to
a verb root ending in an open-syllable the vowel of the suffix will elide. In
the case of these diphthongs, however, this is not true. Thus, as shown in (38),
/ʔáwꜜ-éʃ/ 'say!' is realized as [ʔáwꜜéʃ] and not *[ʔáꜜúʃ].
(38)
|
ʔáꜜwéʃ
|
|
/ʔáwꜜ-éʃ/
|
say-imp.sg
|
'say!'
|
One
final aspect of the Ganza vowel system which should be touched on, if only
briefly, is the presence of vowel harmony. While this system on the whole
cannot be characterised as
"harmonic", there is one context which I have observed thus
far where there is long-distance assimilation of vowel features. This is when
the accusative suffix /-lì/ attaches to the personal pronouns, as shown in (39).
In this environment the default /i/ vowel of the suffix will fully assimilate
with the vowel of the pronoun, or in some cases will elide altogether. For the
second-person plural there is metathesis, but this is anomalous in the
phonology of Ganza.
(39)
|
/tìꜛ-lì/
|
→
|
tìlíꜜ
|
1sg-acc
|
|
/jé-lì/
|
→
|
jélè
|
2sg-acc
|
/kjánáꜜ-lì/
|
→
|
kjánálꜜ
|
3m-acc
|
/kíꜜ-lì/
|
→
|
kílì
~ kílꜜ
|
3f-acc
|
|
/mùꜛ-lì/
|
→
|
mùlúꜜ
|
1pl-acc
|
|
/nàmꜛ-lì/
|
→
|
nàmíl
|
2pl-acc
|
|
/kúꜜ-lì/
|
→
|
kúlù
~ kúlꜜ
|
3pl-acc
|
4.
The Shape of the Word
Having
established the phonemes of Ganza, I now give an account of the structure of
the word, in particular what constitutes a well-formed root. To begin I
describe the attested syllable structures found in free words, making
generalizations about them and giving an example of the maximal syllable. Next
I briefly describe the syllable patterns attested in bound morphemes. Finally,
I look at the distribution patterns of the individual consonant phonemes in
terms of word-position and adjacency in consonant clusters, and from this
derive phonotactic generalizations about the language.
Compared to other Mao languages the syllable patterns attested
in Ganza are notably complex. For example, Bambassi Mao only unambiguously
exhibits CV and CVC syllable profiles (plus their geminate vowel counterparts) in
its monomorphemic words (Ahland 2009:18), whereas Ganza has a wide attestation
of both complex onsets and codas. In Figure 21
below I give examples of all attested syllable profiles in both monosyllabic
and polysyllabic free words in Ganza. Notice especially the complementary
distribution of single and double vowels in open-syllables, with the former
only occurring in polysyllabic words and the latter only in monosyllabic words.
It should also be noted that there are static restrictions for Cw/jV syllables,
with Cj occurring only before /a e o/ and Cw only before /a e i/.
|
Monosyllabic Free Words
|
Polysyllabic Free Words
|
CV
|
not attested
|
|
wá.ꜜsí
|
'meat'
|
CwV
|
not attested
|
|
kʼwà.ɗí
|
'head'
|
CjV
|
not attested
|
|
kjà.lá
|
'Colobus monkey'
|
CVV
|
béè
|
'black kite'
|
not attested
|
|
CwVV
|
pʼwíì
|
'type of tree'
|
not attested
|
|
CjVV
|
kʼjóò
|
'body'
|
not attested
|
|
CVC
|
màk
|
'fox'
|
ʃìl.ká
|
|
CjVC
|
ɲjákʰ
|
'kingfisher'
|
bján.sʼà
|
|
CwVC
|
sʼwék
|
'sorghum'
|
ʃwàm.bà̰
|
'armpit'
|
CVNC
|
ʔúnsʼ
|
'to crawl'
|
kʼónsʼ.ꜜkʼól
|
|
Figure 21 – Syllable profiles of free
words in Ganza.
From these attested profiles we can derive the following generalizations
about well-formed roots in Ganza.
a.
|
"Require
Onset" – All syllables must have an onset. Apparent vowel-initial words
actually begin with the glottal stop phoneme /ʔ/ in the underlying form.
|
|
|
b.
|
"Require
Bimoraic Word" – Free words are minimally bimoraic (i.e. heavy
syllables). Thus all monosyllabic words must have a heavy syllable, with
either a doubled vowel (C(w/j)VV) or a coda (C(w/j)VC).
|
|
|
c.
|
"Permit
Codas" – Simple codas are
permitted in all positions.
|
|
|
d.
|
"Permit
Onglides" – Complex onsets are allowed if the second consonant of the
cluster is a semivowel (Cw/j).
|
|
|
e.
|
"Permit
NC in Rhyme" – Complex codas are allowed if the first consonant of the
cluster is a nasal (CVNC).
|
While the largest syllable found Ganza roots is Cw/jVC or CVNC,
the maximal syllable is actually Cw/jVNC. This occurs when there is final vowel elision
(see §5.1) in a Cw/jVN.CV root. This process
can be seen in (40) and (41), where the root /kʼwántʼà/ 'far' takes the
allomorphic stem /kʼwéntʼꜜ/ when the verbal clitic /=bo/ is attached.
(40)
|
/kʼwántʼà/
|
|
|
|
|
|
(41)
|
/kʼwéntʼꜜ=bo/
|
'[it] is far'
|
|
|
|
|
Unlike free words, bound morphemes in Ganza are all monosyllabic,
never contain double vowels, and may be onsetless. Attested syllable profiles
on suffixes are CV, VC, and V. The CV and VC type suffixes will elide to C,
however, when attached to an open-syllable (i.e. /CV-CV/ à [CV-C] and /CV1-V2C/
à [CV1-C]). The two V suffixes however, namely the
interrogative /-é/ and the locative /-o/, behave in two different ways. The
former elides entirely when attached to an open syllable (i.e. /CV1-é/
à [CV1]) whereas
the latter causes final vowel elision on the stem to which it attaches (i.e.
/CV-o/ à [C-o]). Clitics only
have one attested syllable profile, CV, and never undergo vowel elision (see
also §5.2).
The
following chart (Figure 22) shows the
attested distribution of each consonant phoneme within free word stems in the
following positions: word-initial simple onset, complex onset with /w/, complex
onset with /j/, intervocalic, C1 in a consonant cluster with an oral
consonant, C1 in a consonant cluster with a nasal consonant, C2 in a consonant cluster with an
oral consonant, C2 in a
consonant cluster with a nasal consonant, and word-final simple coda. These data are taken from my analyzed
wordlist, which contains around 1100 entries for free words. It should be noted
that in the chart C stands for oral consonants but does not include the semivowels
/w/ and /j/. A plus symbol (+) indicates that the occurrence is well attested, whereas
a numeral indicates only one or two instances. A minus symbol (-) and dark
shaded cell indicates that the occurrence is not attested in any data. For
consonant clusters, an asterisk (*) indicates that the sequence is not attested
in roots but may be found across morpheme boundaries within stems (e.g. in compounds
or with derivational morphology). Similarly, an exclamation mark (!) indicates
the sequence is not attested in native roots but may be found in borrowed
words. These are last two categories are partially shaded.
|
#_V
|
_w
|
_j
|
V_V
|
_C
|
_N
|
C_
|
N_
|
_#
|
Attested
Consonant Clusters
|
Attested
in Native Roots
|
*Roots,
Morpheme
Boundaries (*)
|
*Roots,
Borrows
(!)
|
/p/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
*
|
!
|
+
|
+
|
pw, pj, mp,
|
pk, pm, np
|
rp
|
/pʼ/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
*
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
pʼw, pʼj, mpʼ
|
pʼt
|
|
/b/
|
+
|
2
|
1
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
!
|
+
|
+
|
bw, bj, mb
|
|
rb
|
/t/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
1/*
|
*/!
|
*/!
|
*/!
|
+
|
tw, tj, tk
|
tp, pt, kt, tm,
tn, nt
|
rt, tn, nt
|
/tʼ/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
*
|
-
|
2
|
+
|
+
|
tʼw, tʼj, ntʼ,
ltʼ, rtʼ, ʔ̃tʼ
|
ttʼ
|
|
/d/
|
+
|
1
|
-
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
dw, nd
|
|
|
/k/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
-
|
+
|
kw, kj, sk, sʼk,
ʃk lk, tk
|
zk, pk, ʃkw,
zkw
|
rk
|
/kʼ/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
kʼw, kʼj, ŋkʼ skʼ,
sʼkʼ, ʃkʼ, lkʼ, nsʼkʼ, jkʼ
|
|
|
/ɡ/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
ɡw, ɡj, ŋɡ, zɡ,
lɡ, ŋɡj
|
mɡ
|
rɡ
|
/m/
|
+
|
+
|
2
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
*/!
|
-
|
+
|
mw, mj, mb, mp,
mpʼ
|
mɡ, mʃ, pm, tm,
lm
|
lm
|
/n/
|
+
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
*/!
|
-
|
+
|
nj, nd, ntʼ,
ns, nsʼ, nz, nsʼkʼ
|
np, nt, tn, nsʼk,
nzj
|
tn, nt
|
/ŋ/
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
ŋkʼ, ŋɡ, ŋɡj
|
|
|
/r/
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
1/!
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
rtʼ
|
|
rp, rb, rt, rk,
rɡ
|
/l/
|
+
|
*
|
|
+
|
+
|
*/!
|
*
|
-
|
+
|
ltʼ, lk, lkʼ,
lɡ
|
lm, lw, ls, ʔl
|
lh, lm
|
/s/
|
+
|
+
|
1
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
*
|
+
|
+
|
sj, sw, ns, sk,
skʼ,
|
ls, ss
|
|
/z/
|
+
|
1
|
+
|
+
|
1/*
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
-
|
zw, zj, zɡ, nz
|
nzj, zkw
|
|
/ʃ/
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
*
|
*
|
+
|
ʃw, ʃk, ʃkʼ
|
wʃ, mʃ, ʃh, ʃkw
|
|
/sʼ/
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
sʼw, sʼj, nsʼ, sʼk,
sʼkʼ, nsʼkʼ
|
|
|
/ʔ/
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
*
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
2
|
|
ʔw, ʔl
|
|
/ʔ̃/
|
-
|
-
|
1
|
+
|
1
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
ʔ̃j, ʔ̃tʼ
|
|
|
/h/
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
1/!
|
-
|
-
|
!/*
|
-
|
-
|
|
ʃh
|
lh
|
/w/
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
*
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
pw, pʼw, bw,
tw, tʼw, dw, kw, kʼw, ɡw, mw, sw, zw, ʃw, sʼw
|
ʔw, lw, wʃ,
ʃkw, zkw
|
|
/j/
|
+
|
-
|
-
|
+
|
2
|
-
|
+
|
+
|
+
|
pj, pʼj, bj,
tj, tʼj, kj, kʼj, ɡj, mj, nj, zj, sj, sʼj, jkʼ, ʔ̃j, ŋɡj
|
nzj
|
|
Figure 22 - Consonant phoneme
distribution patterns of Ganza.
From these data we can make the following
generalizations about Ganza phonotactics:
a.
|
Well-formed
roots do not permit an oral consonant to precede a nasal in consonant
clusters.
|
Except for in borrowed words and at morpheme
boundaries, oral consonants never precede a nasal consonant in a consonant
cluster; *CN is disallowed. This is true both of clusters across a syllable
break and within complex codas.
b.
|
Well-formed
roots do not permit heterorganic places of articulation in nasal-oral
consonant clusters.
|
In Ganza's free word roots all NC clusters are homorganic with
respect to place of articulation, with the exception of semivowel onglides. Since,
however, heterorganic NC and CN clusters are attested across morpheme
boundaries and in borrowed words, there is no active process of place
assimilation.
c.
|
Sibilants
and velar stops have the most liberal distribution patterns.
|
Aside from a few restrictions in consonant clusters, the
phonemes /s, sʼ, kʼ/ and /ɡ/ have the strongest attestation across the
phonotactic environments defined above. Similarly, /k, z/ and /ʃ/ are also well
attested in nearly all environments. They have only a few limitations compared
to their counterparts, namely */ŋk/ (see footnote 15), */ʃj/ and */nʃ/ (which
would be expected if /ʃ/ is indeed an old coalescence of /sj/ as proposed in §2.3),
and the odd lack of /z/ word finally.
d.
|
Velar
oral consonants exhibit the widest range of heterorganic consonant clusters.
|
In particular, the voiceless /k/ and ejective /kʼ/ velar
phonemes are found adjacent to a variety of alveolar consonants, especially as
C2. Also /kʼ/ and /ɡ/ are the
only oral stops found in triadic consonant clusters, namely /nsʼkʼ/ and /ŋɡj/.
e.
|
The
alveolar trill is almost exclusively found in consonant clusters in borrowed
words.
|
All noun and verb roots which attest /r/ in a consonant cluster are
borrowed words, mostly from Arabic. The one exception to this is the word /jàrtʼà/
'arrow', which may or may not be borrowed from a neighbouring language.
In this section I describe two interesting
morphophonemic phenomena found in Ganza. The first is the stem allomorphy of
certain noun and verb stems via final vowel elision and vocalic alternation.
This phenomenon is especially relevant in light of the wide-attestation of final
vowel elision in other Mao languages (Ahland 2009 & Girma 2015). The second
phenomenon is the convergence of final vowel elision, final-devoicing, voicing
assimilation and manner assimilation in stacked suffixes and clitics. As will
be shown, it appears that these processes apply to suffixes, clitics, and roots
at different stages of the phonological derivation, thus lending support to a
serial theory of phonology (e.g. the Theory of Lexical Phonology (Kiparsky 1982,
2000)) rather than a simultaneous theory (e.g. standard Optimality Theory (Prince
& Smolensky 1993)).
Probably the most interesting feature of
Ganza phonology, and one that has already been alluded to in this paper, is the
process of stem allomorphy which occurs in a subset of nouns and verbs in certain
grammatical environments. This stem allomorphy takes two forms in Ganza:
elision of the final vowel, and vocalic alternation. The former is expected
given that final vowel deletion is attested in all other Mao languages (Ahland
2009:16, Girma 2015, Getachew 2015:60). Ganza is unique, however, in that this
elision only occurs in particular grammatical environments and only for a
subset of nouns and verbs.
The second type of allomorphy is a vocalic alternation [ja~e] or [wa~e] and is
also only attested in a subset of nouns and verbs. Hayward wrote about an alternation
between ɑ ~ i attested in several North Omotic languges and questioned whether
the same phenomena is found in the Mao languages (Hayward 1991:539). While
Hayward's alternation and the alternation found in Ganza are phonetically similar,
the two phenomena are not likely related since their locus and conditions are
so divergent, the former affecting verbal inflectional morphemes and the latter
noun and verb roots under certain grammatical conditions.
The two types of stem
allomorphy are not mutually exclusive. Indeed, Ganza noun and verb roots can be
divided into three categories: those which have no allomorphy, those which
undergo final-vowel elision (hereafter FV-Elision), and those which undergo both
FV-Elision and vocalic alternation (hereafter V-Alternation).,
The nouns
are the simplest to describe in terms of the distribution of stem allomorphs.
Simply put, if a noun is modified in any way, whether by a determiner,
adjective, possessor, or the like, it will take its allomorphic form. Examples
of this are given below in (42), (43), and (44) showing the differing behaviour
of nouns with no allomorphy, nouns with FV-Elision, and nouns with FV-Elision
and V-Alternation respectively.
(42)
|
No-allomorphy
|
|
pʼjàlâ
|
'star'
|
wàŋà
|
'chicken'
|
|
pʼjàlátpò
|
'it is a star'
|
wàŋàtpô
|
'it is a chicken'
|
ʔìtʰí pʼjàlàdì
|
'that star'
|
ʔìtʰí wáŋádì
|
'that chicken'
|
náꜜkʰá pʼjàlà
|
|
náꜜkʰá wáŋá
|
|
náꜜkʰá pʼjàlàtpò
|
|
náꜜkʰá wáŋátpò
|
|
(43)
|
FV-Elision
|
|
tʰámâ
|
'fire'
|
kʰálà
|
'porridge'
|
|
tʰámátpò
|
'it is a fire'
|
kʰálàtpò
|
'it is a porridge'
|
ʔìtʰí tʰámdî
|
'that fire'
|
ʔìtʰí kʰállì
|
'that porridge'
|
náꜜkʰá tʰám
|
|
kʰárá kʰál
|
|
náꜜkʰá tʰámdìpò
|
|
kʰárá kʰállìpò
|
|
(44)
|
FV-Elision & V-Alternation
|
|
tʼjámà
|
'grave'
|
wáŋà
|
'ensete'
|
|
tʼjámàtpò
|
'it is a grave'
|
wáŋàtpò
|
'it is ensete'
|
ʔìtʰí
tʼémdì
|
'that grave'
|
ʔìtʰí wéŋdì
|
'that ensete'
|
náꜜkʰá tʼém
|
|
náꜜkʰá wéŋ
|
|
náꜜkʰá tʼémdìpò
|
|
náꜜkʰá wéŋdìbò
|
|
This behavior can also be seen
in compounds, where all morphemes except the head (leftmost) will take an
allomorphic form. For example the word for 'West' /ʔábà-ɡìz-kwéè/ [ʔábàɡìzkwéè]
is a three-word compound from the words /ʔábà/ 'sun', /ɡìzá/ 'to enter',
and /kwájà/ 'place'. The second morpheme undergoes FV-Elision from /ɡìzá/
to /ɡìz/, and the third morpheme undergoes both FV-Elision and V-Alternation
from /kwájà/ to /kwéè/. Furthermore, if another modifier is added, such as
a demonstrative pronoun, the head noun will also take its allomorphic form. For
example, taking the words /kárá/ 'hot' and /kálà/ 'porridge', the phrase
/kárá kálꜜ/ [kárá kál] can be formed and the second morpheme takes its allomorph.
When the proximal demonstrative /ʔìntìꜛ/ is appended the phrase becomes /ʔìntìꜛ
kàr kálꜜ/ [ʔìntì kár ꜜkál] 'this hot porridge' and both morphemes take
their allomorphic form.
The verb system presents a
much more complex pattern with regard to stem allomorphy. In Figure 23 I give the environments I have elicited
to-date in which the full stem of a verb is used and in which the allomorphic
stem is used.
|
|
Conditional
|
/-n/
|
VC1
|
/=bo/
|
Non-Final
SS
|
/-p/
|
VC2
|
/=na/
|
Non-Final
DS
|
/-l/
|
Nominalized
|
/-di/
|
Serial
verb
|
-
|
Imperative
|
/-éʃ/,
/-èm/
|
Jussive
|
/kwámàn/
|
Future
|
/-sa/
|
Negative
|
/-ánꜜ/
|
Purpose
|
/-sa/
|
Neg.
Imperative
|
/-ánꜜ/ +
/-éʃ/, /-èm/
|
Polar
Question
|
-
|
Perfect
|
/-ɡwáꜜ/
|
Content
Question
|
/-e/
|
|
|
Relative
Clause
|
/-di/
|
|
|
Continuous
auxiliary
|
/ɡàráꜜ/
|
|
|
Reason
|
/-di/ +
/kódò/
|
Figure 23- Environments
for stem allomorphy in Ganza verbs.
It is difficult to determine
if there is an overarching semantic or grammatical condition for when a stem will
take its full form or take its allomorph, and I have no definite opinion. With
both negatives, conditionals, and jussives using the full stem and
interrogatives, futures, and purpose using the allomorphic stem, a realis-irrealis
division is unlikely. A negative-affirmative division is also obviously untenable.
My current hypothesis is that this is in fact a morphological condition, not a
grammatical or semantic one.
The following examples in (45)
(46) and (47) show the patterns for each of the three types of verbs: those
with no-allomorphy, FV-Elision, and FV-Elision plus V-Alternation respectively.
(45)
|
No allomorphy:
/pʼápʼí/ 'to gather'
|
|
VC1
|
hàɡá pʼábḭ́bô
|
'he gathers'
|
|
VC2
|
hàɡá pʼàbḭ̀nà
|
'he gathers'
|
|
Negative
|
pʼábḭ́ánnà
|
'not gather'
|
|
Nominalized
|
pʼábḭ́tká ákʰúmbô
|
'it is good to gather'
|
|
Imperative
|
pʼábḭ́ʃ
|
'gather!'
|
|
Neg-Imperative
|
pʼábḭ́áꜜnéʃ
|
'don't gather!'
|
|
Conditional
|
hàná pʼàbḭ̀nɡà
ákʰúmbô
|
'it is good if you gather'
|
|
Non-Final
|
hàɡú pʼábḭ́pɡú
kʰwáꜜábô
|
'having gathered they came'
|
|
Serial
verb
|
hàdí pʼábḭ́
ʃíꜜníbô
|
'I gathered and worked'
|
|
Perfect
|
hàdí pʼábḭ́ɡwábò
|
'I have gathered'
|
|
Future
|
pʼàbḭ̀smùbô
|
'we will gather'
|
|
Purpose
|
pʼàbḭ̀ssì kʰwáꜜábô
|
'I come to gather'
|
|
Polar
Question
|
hàmá
pʼàbḭ̀?
|
'are you gathering?'
|
|
Content
Question
|
nómà
pʼábḭ́?
|
'what are you gathering?'
|
|
Relative
Clause
|
pʼàbḭ̀ ábàtkà kʰwáꜜábô
|
'the day of gathering is come'
|
|
Jussive
|
kʰwámàn
kʰwàɡá pʼábḭ́
|
'let us gather our pumpkins!'
|
(46)
|
FV-Elision:
/káꜜpá/ 'to take'
|
|
VC1
|
hàɡá kʰápꜜbô
|
'he takes'
|
|
VC2
|
hàɡá kʰápnà
|
'he takes'
|
|
Negative
|
káꜜpʰáánnà
|
'not take'
|
|
Nominalized
|
kʰápꜜtíɡá ákʰúmbô
|
'it is good to take'
|
|
Imperative
|
kʰáꜜpʰéʃ
|
'take!'
|
|
NegImperative
|
kʰáꜜpʰááꜜnéʃ
|
'don't take!'
|
|
Conditional
|
hàná kʰápʰànɡà
ákúmbó
|
'It is good if you take'
|
|
NonFinal
|
hàɡú kʰáꜜpʰápɡú
kʰwáꜜábô
|
'having taken they came'
|
|
Serial
verb
|
hàdí kʰáꜜpʰá
háwbô
|
'I take and go'
|
|
Perfect
|
hàdí kʰáꜜpʰáɡwábò
|
'I have taken'
|
|
Future
|
kʰápʰsàdìbô
|
'I will take'
|
|
Purpose
|
kʰápʰsàdì kʰwáꜜábô
|
'I come to take'
|
|
Polar
Question
|
hàná kʰápʰ?
|
'are you taking (it)?'
|
|
Content
Question
|
nónà kʰáꜜpʰé?
|
'what are you taking?'
|
|
Relative
Clause
|
kʰápʰ
ꜜábàtkà kʰwáꜜábô
|
'the day of taking is come'
|
|
Jussive
|
kʰwámàn kʰáꜜpʰá
|
'let us take!'
|
(47)
|
FV-Elision
& V-Alternation: /wáná/ 'to thread (beads)'
|
|
VC1
|
hàɡá wénbô
|
'he threads'
|
|
VC2
|
hàɡá wènnà
|
'he threads'
|
|
Negative
|
wánáánnà
|
'not thread'
|
|
Nominalized
|
wéndíɡà ákʰúmbô
|
'it is good to thread'
|
|
Imperative
|
wénéʃ
|
'thread!'
|
|
NegImperative
|
wánááꜜnéʃ
|
'don't thread!'
|
|
Conditional
|
hàná wànànɡà
ákúmbó
|
'it is good if you thread'
|
|
NonFinal
|
hàɡú wánápɡú
kʰwáꜜábô
|
'having threaded they came'
|
|
Serial
verb
|
hàdí wáná
sʼjérbò
|
'I thread and sew'
|
|
Perfect
|
hàdí wánáɡwábò
|
'I have threaded'
|
|
Future
|
wènsàdìbô
|
'I will thread'
|
|
Purpose
|
wènsàdì kʰwáꜜábô
|
'I come to thread'
|
|
Polar
Question
|
hàná wèn?
|
'are you threading?'
|
|
Content
Question
|
nónà wéné?
|
'what are you threading?'
|
|
Relative
Clause
|
wèn ábàtkà kʰwáꜜábô
|
'the day of threading is come'
|
|
Jussive
|
kʰwámàn mù sã́ʔĩ̀ wáná
|
'let us thread our beads!'
|
Another interesting phenomenon in Ganza
morphophonemics is the interaction between the processes of vowel elision, voicing
and manner assimilation, and word-final devoicing and deglottalization. As has
been illustrated already in §2.1, voiced oral stops devoice word-finally and
ejective oral stops deglottalize. Also illustrated was the rightward-spreading
voicing assimilation in consonant clusters formed at a morpheme boundary. These
were discussed in terms of identifying the underlying voiced or ejective
feature of a word-final /b/ /d/ and /ɡ/ versus /pʼ/ /tʼ/ and /kʼ/. In addition,
in §2.6 I discussed how oral alveolar consonants which are adjacent at a
morpheme boundary display rightward spreading manner assimilation, creating a
long or doubled consonant. These processes are highly productive and often
complementary or concurrent. They also interact with another process of vowel elision,
different from that described in §5.1 but mentioned briefly in §4.1, in which
certain suffixes elide their vowel if attached to an open-syllable (hereafter
SV-Elision). The interplay of these processes results in some interesting
situations when suffixes and clitics are stacked. Here the language appears to
deal with suffixes, clitics, and roots at different stages of the phonological
derivation. I discuss two of these situations below.
The first case is with the nominal
marker /-di/. When affixed to a closed syllable the initial consonant of the
/-di/ suffix will assimilate the underlying voice feature of the final
consonant of its host. Thus with underlyingly voiceless consonants /-di/ is
realized as [ti] (48a), with underlyingly voiced oral consonants it is realized
as [di] (48b), with underlyingly ejective consonants it is realized as [ti] or
[tʼi] if the host is alveolar (48c), and with nasal consonants it is realized
as [di] (48d). As mentioned in §2.1, because of this voicing assimilation the
addition of the /-di/ suffix reveals the underlying contrast of voiced and
ejective phonemes, which would otherwise be neutralized word-finally due to
deglotalization and devoicing, (c.f. column 1 in (48b) and (48c)). However,
because of deglottalization in this context ejective and voiceless are now
neutralized for non-alveolar consonant clusters.
(48)
|
a.
|
/-di/ assimilates voicelessness with underlyingly
voiceless consonants.
|
|
|
/p/
|
ʔápʰ
|
'eye'
|
ʔápti
|
'the/an
eye'
|
|
|
/t/
|
bàtʰ
|
'duck'
|
bàttí
|
'the/a duck'
|
|
|
/k/
|
swàkʰ
|
'spirit'
|
swàktí
|
'the/a
spirit'
|
|
|
|
|
|
b.
|
/-di/ retains voicing with underlyingly voiced oral
consonants.
|
|
|
/b/
|
dôp
|
'lion'
|
dóbdì
|
'the/a lion'
|
|
|
/d/
|
kʰìkʰìmít
|
'ground hornbill'
|
kʰìkʰìmíddì
|
'the/a
ground hornbill'
|
|
|
/ɡ/
|
ʔôk
|
'hat'
|
ʔóɡdì
|
'the/a hat'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
c.
|
/-di/ assimilates
voicelessness with underlyingly ejective consonants.
|
|
|
/pʼ/
|
ʃwàʃàp
|
'tarantula'
|
ʃwàʃàptí
|
'the/a tarantula'
|
|
|
/tʼ/
|
ʃêt
|
'buffalo'
|
ʃéttʼì
|
'the/a buffalo'
|
|
|
/kʼ/
|
màk
|
'fox'
|
màktí
|
'the/a fox'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
d.
|
/-di/ retains voicing with underlyingly nasal
consonants.
|
|
|
/m/
|
ɡùɾùm
|
'wild pig'
|
ɡùɾùmdì
|
'the/a
wild pig'
|
|
|
/n/
|
kʰàlmàn
|
'camel'
|
kʰàlmàndì
|
'the/a camel'
|
|
|
/ŋ/
|
kàrùŋ
|
'tent'
|
kàrùŋdí
|
'the/a tent'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
In addition to voicing
assimilation, when /-di/ is attached to a word ending in an oral alveolar
consonant there is also full manner assimilation. Example (26) is here
reiterated as (49).
(49)
|
|
Isolation
|
Manner assimilation of
/-di/ suffix
|
|
/r/
|
ɡábîr
|
'sheep'
|
ɡábírrì
|
'the/a sheep'
|
|
/l/
|
jàwǐl
|
'hyena'
|
jàwìllí
|
'the/a hyena'
|
|
/t/
|
hàdìtʰ
|
'metal'
|
hàdìttʰí
|
'the/a metal'
|
|
/d/
|
kʰìkʰìmít
|
'ground hornbill'
|
kʰìkʰìmíddì
|
'the/a ground
hornbill'
|
|
/tʼ/
|
ʃêt
|
'buffalo'
|
ʃéttʼì
|
'the/a buffalo'
|
|
/s/
|
ɡàmìs
|
'shirt'
|
ɡàmìssí
|
'the/a shirt'
|
|
/ʃ/
|
kʼóꜜŋóʃ
|
'hunchback'
|
kʼóꜜŋóʃʃí
|
'the/a hunchback'
|
|
/sʼ/
|
ɡíŋɡìlísʼ
|
'lovebird'
|
ɡíŋɡìlíssʼí
|
'the/a lovebird'
|
When /-di/ is attached to a word
ending in a vowel, however, there is SV-Elision and devoicing so that /-di/ is
realized as a coda [t] (50) (see footnote 37 regarding exceptions).
(50)
|
SV-Elision and devoicing of /-di/ suffix.
|
|
ɡáŋát
|
|
/ɡáŋá-di/
|
|
donkey-nm
|
|
'the/a
donkey'
|
When two suffixes are stacked,
such as the nominal marker /-di/ and the accusative marker /-lì/, SV-Elision
and devoicing is blocked for the first suffix and applied to the second (devoicing
only with oral stops), as shown in (51).
(51)
|
SV-Elision and devoicing of /-di/ suffix blocked and
SV-Elision of /-lì/ suffix applied.
|
|
ɡáŋádíl
|
|
/ɡáŋá-di-lì/
|
|
donkey-nm-acc
|
|
'the/a
donkey (accusative)'
|
On the other hand, when a
clitic is stacked on top of the /-di/ suffix a curious thing happens. Instead
of the SV-Elision and devoicing being blocked by the addition of a word-final
syllable, as would be expected, they both apply and the voiceless feature of
the now elided and devoiced /-t/ spreads to the initial consonant of the
clitic. Two examples of this are given in (52) and (53).
(52)
|
SV-Elision and devoicing of /-di/ suffix followed by voiceless spread
to /=bo/ clitic.
|
|
ɡáŋátpô
|
|
/ɡáŋá-di=bo/
|
|
donkey-nm=vc1
|
|
'it is
a donkey'
|
(53)
|
SV-Elision and devoicing of /-di/ suffix followed by voiceless
spread to /=ɡa/ clitic.
|
|
ɡáŋátká
|
náꜜkʰábô
|
|
/ɡáŋá-di=ɡa
|
náꜜká=bo/
|
|
donkey-nm=3m.sbj
|
big=vc1
|
|
'the donkey is big'
|
|
When two suffixes and a clitic
are stacked, SV-Elision and devoicing is blocked for the first suffix and
applied to the second, then voicing and manner are spread from the second
suffix to the initial consonant of the clitic. This process can be seen when
the nominal clause marking suffix /-di/, the accusative suffix /-lì/, and the
first-person pronominal subject clitic /-di/ are stacked, as shown in (54).
(54)
|
SV-Elision of /-lì/ suffix followed by manner
spread to /-di/ clitic.
|
|
ɡáŋádíllì
|
kʰwésʼpô
|
|
/ɡáŋá-di-lì=di
|
kwésʼ=bo/
|
|
donkey-nm-acc=1sg.sbj
|
hit=vc1
|
|
'I hit
the donkey'
|
|
Several questions arise from
these data: if /ɡáŋá-di=ɡa/ is realized as [ɡáŋátká], then why is /dôb=ɡa/
realized as as [dóbɡà] instead of *[dópkà]. Similarly, why isn't
/ɡáŋá-di-lì-di/ realized as *[ɡáŋádílìt], *[ɡáŋáttìdì], or *[ɡáŋáddìdì]?
While it is not my intention to present a full analysis here, I would suggest
that in order to account for this there must be a serial application of these
phonological processes. In the first stage of the derivation affixes are added
and elided and devoiced at the word boundary. Next clitics are added and
voice-manner is spread. Finally, if no affixes or clitics have been added in
the previous cycles, there is devoicing and deglottalization on roots. This
schema is illustrated in (55).
(55)
|
Suffix
FV-Elision & Devoicing >> Voice &Manner Spread to Clitics
>> Root Devoicing
|
A similar effect is seen with
the directional suffix /-sa/. Like the nominal marker /-di/, when /-sa/ is
attached to a word ending in a vowel the final vowel of the suffix is elided,
but when attached to a word ending in a consonant SV-Elision is blocked. For
this morpheme, however, SV-Elision
also appears to be blocked word-finally and thus when elicited in isolation /wáꜜmá-sa
/ is realized as [wáꜜmásá]. These cases are illustrated in (56).
(56)
|
a.
|
CVC
noun + /-sa/
|
b.
|
CVCV noun + /-sa/
|
|
|
súksà
|
|
wáꜜmásá
|
|
|
/súkꜜ-sa/
|
|
/wáꜜmá-sa/
|
|
|
shop-dir
|
|
river-dir
|
|
|
'to
the shop'
|
|
'to
the river'
|
When a clitic is added to
these two environments, stacking on the suffix, SV-Elision and voice
assimilation occur in the one but are blocked in the other, as shown in (57).
If the nominal clitic happens to start with an alveolar consonant there is also
manner assimilation, as in (58).
(57)
|
a.
|
CVC noun + /-sa/ + /=ɡa/ à [saɡa]
|
b.
|
CVCV noun + /-sa/ + /=ɡa/ à [ska]
|
|
|
súksàɡà
|
hááꜜɡwábò
|
|
wáꜜmáská
|
hááꜜɡwábò
|
|
|
/súkꜜ-sa=ɡa
|
hááꜜ-ɡwáꜜ=bo/
|
|
/wáꜜmá-sa=ɡa
|
hááꜜ-ɡwáꜜ=bo/
|
|
|
shop-dir=3m.sbj
|
go-pfv=vc1
|
|
river-dir=3m.sbj
|
go-pfv=vc1
|
|
|
'he has
gone to the shop'
|
|
'he
has gone to the river'
|
(58)
|
a.
|
CVC
noun + /-sa/ + /=di/
|
b.
|
CVCV noun +
/-sa/ + /=di/
|
|
|
súksàdì
|
hááꜜɡwábò
|
|
wáꜜmássí
|
hááꜜɡwábò
|
|
|
/súkꜜ-sa=di
|
hááꜜ-ɡwáꜜ=bo/
|
|
/wáꜜmá-sa=di
|
hááꜜ-ɡwáꜜ=bo/
|
|
|
shop-dir=1sg.sbj
|
go-pfv=vc1
|
|
river-dir-3m
|
go-pfv=vc1
|
|
|
'I
have gone to the shop'
|
|
'he
has gone to the river'
|
This is also the case with the
future construction. Here the directional marker /-sa/ is repurposed as a
future tense marker by attaching to a verb stem along with a subject clitic. If
the verb takes an allomorphic form, thus ending in a consonant, the full form
of both morphemes is preserved, as in (59a). If the verb stem ends in a vowel,
however, there is SV-Elision and voice assimilation to the initial consonant of
the clitic as in (59b). A full paradigm of /-sa/ with the subject
clitics is given in (60).
(59)
|
a.
|
CVC
Verb + /-sa/ + /=ɡi/
|
b.
|
CVCV Verb + /-sa/ + /=ɡi/
|
|
|
tʼélsáɡìbô
|
|
pʼàbḭ̀skìbô
|
|
|
/tʼélꜜ-sa=ɡi=ꜛ=bo/
|
|
/pʼàpʼì-sa=ɡi=ꜛ=bo/
|
|
|
seek-fut=3f.sbj=irr=vc1
|
|
seek-fut=3f.sbj=irr=vc1
|
|
|
'she
will search'
|
|
'she
will gather'
|
(60)
|
Forms of the directional-future suffix /-sa/ combined with the subject
clitics.
|
|
sadi ~ ssi
|
dir/fut=1sg.sbj
|
samu ~ smu
|
dir/fut=1pl.sbj
|
|
sana ~ sna
|
dir/fut=2sg.sbj
|
sama ~ sma
|
dir/fut=2pl.sbj
|
|
saɡa ~ ska
|
dir/fut=3m.sbj
|
saɡu ~ sku
|
dir/fut=3pl.sbj
|
|
saɡi ~ ski
|
dir/fut=3f.sbj
|
|
|
Similar questions are raised
from these data as with those of the previous case study. If /wáꜜmá-di-lì/
'the river (accusative)' is realized [wáꜜmádíl] why is /wáꜜmá-sa-di/ 'to
the river + I (subject)' realized as [wáꜜmássí] and not *[wáꜜmását]? What
would motivate SV-Elision in the second stacked morpheme in the former case but
of the first morpheme in the latter?
As the least these two case
studies evidence that the phonology of Ganza treats suffixes, clitics, and
roots differently in different environments. There are at least two types of final vowel
elision: FV-Elision which applies to roots in certain grammatical environments,
and SV-Elision which applies to suffixes in certain phonological environments.
Clitics are exempted from both types of elision. There are also at least two
types of devoicing: devoicing that applies after SV-Elision and which generates
a spreadable voiceless feature, and word-final devoicing which occurs after the
application of morphology. These phenomena lend considerable support to a
serial theory of the application of phonological processes.
In this final section of the paper I give a
condensed account of the Ganza tone system. The following discussion will
include an overview of the phonemic tone levels and functional load of tone (§6.1),
an overview of basic tonal phenomena which account for tonal behaviour (§6.2),
and a description and exemplification of the tonal melodies found on both noun roots (§6.3) and verb roots (§6.4).
For the purposes of my description I will assume such things as the
autosegmental nature of tone and the existence of tonal register.
Ganza has two
phonemic tones or tonemes, high (H)
and low (L), both of which can occur as either associated or unassociated
(floating) tones in the underlying form. A floating L will cause downstep (also called non-automatic downstep) of a following H,
and is attested root-internally as well as between word and morpheme boundaries.
I use the symbol (ꜛ) to indicate a floating H in the underlying form, and (ꜜ)
to indictate a floating L in the underlying form as well as downstep in the
surface form. Unlike a floating L, I have not yet observed that a floating H
causes any significant change in the tonal register; it either attaches to an available tone-bearing unit (TBU) or is deleted.
Contour tones also exist, both falling and rising, but these are only ever
realized on bimoraic syllables (i.e. double vowels or utterance-final
lengthened vowels) and are best treated as two separate tonemes on adjacent TBUs.
The approximate pitch
difference between the tonemes when realised on the same or different registers
is illustrated in Figure 24 below. The
difference between a H and a L on the same register is approximately 30Hz. Register
shift in Ganza is 10Hz on average, and thus the difference between H and L on a
lowered register is approximately 40Hz, and between H and ꜜH is 10Hz. Since
Ganza has downdrift (also called automatic downstep) in addition to downstep,
there is no contrastive height difference between the sequences LH and LꜜH, as
both would be realized as a LH on the same register. These measurements
represent the average for a short utterance, and may change significantly over
a longer utterance with multiple changes in register.
Figure 24 - Approximate height difference between Ganza
tone levels in Hertz (Hz).
Tone carries a high lexical
functional load in Ganza. It is a contrastive feature of noun and verb roots,
as the tonal minimal pairs in (61) illustrate. It is also contrastive in various
functional morphemes, for example the question morphemes /nòꜛ/ 'where' and
/nóꜜ/ 'what' in (62), and the medial and distal demonstrative pronouns /ʔìtìꜛ/
'that (near)' and /ʔìtí/ 'that (far)' in (63).
(61)
|
/Lꜛ/
|
wàŋà
|
'chicken'
|
cf.
|
/HL/
|
wáŋà
|
'ensete'
|
|
/HL/
|
wáɗà
|
'to
insult'
|
|
/HꜜH/
|
wáꜜɗá
|
'to
distribute'
|
|
/Lꜛ/
|
wàlà
|
'to
remain'
|
|
/LH/
|
wàlá
|
'to seem'
|
(62)
|
nòɡú
|
pʰòlò
|
cf.
|
nóɡù
|
pʰòlò
|
|
/nòꜛ=ɡu
|
pòlò/
|
|
/nóꜜ=
|
ɡu
pòlò/
|
|
where=3pl.sbj
|
sit
|
|
what=3pl.sbj
|
sit
|
|
|
|
(63)
|
ʔìtʰì
|
ɡúrùmdì
|
cf.
|
ʔìtʰí
|
ɡùrùmdì
|
|
/ʔìtìꜛ /
|
ɡùrùm-di
|
|
/ʔìtí
|
ɡùrùm-di/
|
|
dem.med
|
boar-nm
|
|
dem.dist
|
boar-nm
|
|
|
|
I have a strong suspicion that
tone also has a considerable grammatical load in Ganza, though more research is
needed to determine the specifics. Up to now the only grammatical tone I have
been able to observe with confidence is the L boundary tone used in the
indicative mood, and possibly an
irrealis H tone (see example (59) and footnote 38).
Contrasting tonal melodies on complementary morphemes might also be considered
grammatical tone, such as the imperative suffixes /-éʃ/ 'imp.sg' and /-èm/ 'imp.pl' which have opposing /H/ and /L/ melodies.
This is illustrated in (64) with the verb /ɡìzá/ (here taking the allomorphic
form /ɡìzꜛ/).
(64)
|
ɡìzéʃ
|
cf.
|
ɡìzêm
|
|
/ɡìzꜛ-éʃ/
|
|
/ɡìzꜛ-èm/
|
|
enter-imp.sg
|
|
enter-imp.pl
|
|
|
|
Before describing the underlying melodies
attested on noun and verb roots several foundational tonal phenomena need to be
explained. These phenomena account for the various surface pitch permutations
of the underlying melodies. They are as follows:
a. Downdrift
Ganza exhibits downdrift,
meaning that an overt L will cause a following H to be realized on a lower
register than any previous H. Thus, in the sequence /HLH/ the second H will
have a surface pitch approximately 10Hz lower than that of the first. Downstep
in Ganza is also "terracing" (Snider 1999), meaning that after a
downstepped H a L will also be realized on a lower register than a preceding L.
Again, in the sequence /LHL/ the second L will be realized roughly 10Hz lower
than the first. As multiple downsteps are applied there is a downward
"terracing" effect on the overall register of the utterance. This
effect can be seen clearly when multiple /HL/ melody words are placed adjacent
in a single phrase, as in (65) (the first data line is a spatial representation
of the relative heights of surface pitch).
(65)
|
|
|
|
ʔìtʰí
|
nã́ʔĩ̀
|
ʔápʰà
|
kʰáʔà
|
|
/ʔìtí
|
náʔ̃ì
|
ʔápʰà
|
káʔà/
|
|
dem.dist
|
daughter
|
uncle
|
house
|
|
'that (far) daughter's
uncle's house'
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
b. Construct tone
melodies.
In addition to a citation tone melody, every noun,
adjective, and verb in Ganza has an alternative tone melody which it takes in
certain grammatical environments, which I will refer to as the construct melody. For nouns and
adjectives this alternative melody is applied when the word is modified,
whether by another noun, a demonstrative, a possessive pronoun, etc. This
pattern corresponds exactly with that of nominal stem allomorphy described in §5.1.
For verbs the construct melody is taken in differing grammatical environments (see
§6.4 below), but unlike the nouns these environments do not correspond exactly to
those of the verb stem allomorphy.
This phenomenon was first
observed by Michael Ahland in the phonology of Bambassi Mao (2009). The term
"construct melody" was first used in his dissertation (Ahland 2012)
and was recommended to me by him. A similar phenomenon has also been observed by
Anne-Christie Hellenthal in Sheko, a Majoid-Omotic language (2010:123). It will
be interesting to see if this feature is also attested in the other Mao
languages, Hoozo and Sezo, as this will have significant bearing on the
reconstruction of the Proto-Mao tone system.
c. Toneless TBUs.
Many of Ganza's bound
morphemes exhibit toneless behaviour, including most nominal suffixes and all
clitics. Underlyingly these morphemes have an empty TBU, and thus their surface
tone is entirely dependent on the melody of the preceding tone-bearing
morpheme. These toneless morphemes contrast with bound verbal morphemes such as
the perfect /-ɡwáꜜ/~/-ɡáꜜ/, the negative /-ánꜜ/, and the imperatives /-éʃ/ 'imp.sg' and /-èm/ 'imp.pl', which have melodies of their
own.
d. Rightward-unbounded
tone spread within the phonological word.
An underlying associated H or
L tone of a noun or verb root in Ganza will spread rightward until blocked by
another overt tone. Thus, a tone will associate with all toneless morphemes
immediately to the right of the original TBU until it meets a TBU already
associated with a tone. This spreading does not occur across word boundaries,
but it will occur freely across both suffix and clitic morpheme boundaries.
Thus the phonological word in this
context is considered to be a headword and all its subsequent suffixes and
clitics.
e. Boundary tones.
Ganza exhibits an abundance of
floating tones which tend to associate with the phonological word at phrase and
utterance boundaries. For example the boundary L (mentioned in §6.1) floats at
the end of an indicative utterance and, in conjunction with utterance-final
lengthening, produces a falling contour if adjacent to a H. Also, many of the
noun and verb melodies evidence floating tones which may surface as contour
tones, downstep, fully realized level tones, or delete depending on the
morphological context.
These last three phenomena–
toneless morphemes, rightward-unbounded tone spread, and boundary tones– can be
illustrated with a single example, as in (66). Here the toneless morphemes
/-di/ 'nm' and /=ɡa/ '3m.sbj' are suffixed to the /H/ melody
word /kúzár/ 'domestic pig', followed by the /LH/ verb /pùbá/ 'sick' and
the toneless verbal clitic /=bo/. The H of /kúzár/ spreads to the toneless
/-di/ and /=ɡa/ before being blocked by the initial L of /pùbá/. The H of
/pùbá/ spreads to /=bo/, after which utterance-final lengthening generates an
extra mora to which the floating boundary L associates, resulting in a falling
surface tone.
(66)
|
kʰúzárríɡá
|
pʰùbábô
|
|
|
/kúzár-di=ɡa
|
pùbá=bo/
|
|
pig-nm=3m.sbj
|
sick=vc1
|
|
'the pig is sick'
|
Based on their pairings of citation and
construct melodies, Ganza simplex nouns (i.e. disyllabic monomorphemic nouns)
can be divided into ten different lexical
tone classes (hereafter LTCs). There are eight attested citation melodies— /H/,
/Hꜜ/, /HꜜH/, /HL/, /LH/, /LHꜜ/, /L/, and /Lꜛ/—
and three attested construct melodies— /L/, /HL/, and /Hꜜ/. Six of the
eight citation melodies predictably pair with only one of the construct
melodies; the two remaining citation melodies each may take one of two
construct melodies. The resulting ten LTCs are illustrated in Figure 25 below. Note that for the rest of this paper
I will represent LTCs with phonemic forward-slashes and a colon, with the
citation melody to the left of the colon and the construct melody to the right.
Noun-LTCs
|
|
/L/
|
/HL/
|
/Hꜜ/
|
Citation Melodies
|
/H/
|
/H:L/
|
|
|
/Hꜜ/
|
/Hꜜ:L/
|
|
/Hꜜ:Hꜜ/
|
/HꜜH/
|
|
/HꜜH:HL/
|
/HꜜH:Hꜜ/
|
/HL/
|
|
/HL:HL/
|
|
/LH/
|
/LH:L/
|
|
|
/LHꜜ/
|
/LHꜜ:L/
|
|
|
/L/
|
/L:L/
|
|
|
/Lꜛ/
|
|
|
/Lꜛ:Hꜜ/
|
Figure 25 – Nominal lexical
tone classes in Ganza based on citation and construct tone melody pairings.
In Figure 26 I give examples of each nominal LTC, first
showing the underlying form of the word and then exemplifying the various
surface forms of both of its citation and construct melodies. The frames given
are first isolation, which shows the citation melody plus utterance-final
lengthening, second suffixing of the plural nominal marker /-ɡu/, which shows
the citation melody extended onto a toneless morpheme, and third the addition
of the plural distal demonstrative /ʔùɡú/ (plus an obligatory /-ɡu/ 'nm.pl' suffix), which shows the construct melody by modifying the noun.
LTC
|
Underlying Form
(Citation : Construct)
|
Isolation
(Citation)
|
Plural Nominal Marker
(Citation)
|
Plural Distal Demonstrative
(Construct)
|
/H:L/
|
/ɡáŋá
: ɡàŋà/
|
'donkey'
|
ɡáŋá
|
ɡáŋáɡú
|
ʔùɡú
ɡàŋàɡù
|
/Hꜜ:Hꜜ/
|
/sásóꜜ/
|
'monkey'
|
sásô
|
sásóɡù
|
ʔùɡú sásóɡù
|
/Hꜜ:L/
|
/tʼúláꜜ : tʼùlà/
|
'dikdik'
|
tʼúlâ
|
tʼúláɡù
|
ʔùɡú tʼùlàɡù
|
/HꜜH:Hꜜ/
|
/káꜜná : kánáꜜ/
|
'dog'
|
kʰáꜜná
|
kʰáꜜnáɡú
|
ʔùɡú kʰánáɡù
|
/HꜜH:HL/
|
/sáꜜʔá
: sáʔà/
|
'ɡoat'
|
sáꜜʔá
|
sáꜜʔáɡú
|
ʔùɡú
sáʔàɡù
|
/HL:HL/
|
/pátʼà/
|
'deer'
|
pʰáɗà
|
pʰáɗàɡù
|
ʔùɡú pʰáɗàɡù
|
/LH:L/
|
/mìmí
: mìmì/
|
'mosquito'
|
mìmí
|
mìmíɡú
|
ʔùɡú
mìmìɡù
|
/LHꜜ:L/
|
/kjàláꜜ
: kjàlà/
|
'colobus'
|
kʰjàlâ
|
kʰjàláɡù
|
ʔùɡú
kʰjàlàɡù
|
/L:L/
|
/kùrù/
|
'genet'
|
kʰùrù
|
kʰùrùɡù
|
ʔùɡú
kʰùrùɡù
|
/Lꜛ:Hꜜ/
|
/wàŋàꜛ
: wáŋáꜜ/
|
'chicken'
|
wàŋà
|
wàŋàɡú
|
ʔùɡú
wáŋáɡù
|
Figure 26 - Examples of
nominal lexical tone classes on disyllabic words.
In isolation it can be seen
that a floating L (ꜜ) results in a falling contour tone (e.g. /sásóꜜ/ à
[sásôː]). This can be attributed to utterance-final lengthening, which adds a
mora to the final vowel of the noun root and allows the floating L to associate
and create a contour. A floating H (ꜛ) does not consistantly produce this
effect, however (e.g. /wàŋàꜛ/ à [wàŋà ~ wàŋǎː] cf. footnote 40). This is part of
a larger pattern in the tonology where a
floating H will delete at certain boundaries if there is no fully realized TBU
for it to associate with, contrasting with a floating L which will consistantly
result in a contour or downstep if not fully associated.
The plural nominal marker frame
reveals floating tones by providing an empty TBU for them to associate with
(e.g. /sásóꜜ-ɡu/ à [sásóɡù], /wàŋàꜛ-ɡu/ à [wàŋàɡú]). LTCs
without a floating tone will simply spread the rightmost lexical tone to this
empty TBU (e.g. /mìmí-ɡu/ à [mìmíɡú]). Consequently, LTCs whose citation
melodies only differ by a floating tone can be clearly differentiated here (cf.
/H/ and /Hꜜ/, /LH/ and /LHꜜ/, /L/ and /Lꜛ/).
Finally, the plural distal
demonstrative frame differentiates LTCs with the same citation melody but
different construct melodies (cf. /Hꜜ:Hꜜ/ and /Hꜜ:L/, /HꜜH:Hꜜ/ and /HꜜH:HL/).
Nouns in these LTCs behave alike in environments utilizing the citation melody,
but their behaviours diverge when modified by another word.
The above frames all show the
citation and construct melodies with empty TBUs in the righthand environment.
When the immediate righthand environment is occupied by an overt tone, however,
a new set of phenomena occurs. The simple possessive construction is a helpful
frame for seeing these phenomena and will be used in the examples to follow.
As can be expected, when two
identical tonemes are placed adjacent to one another they merge in response to
the Obligatory Contour Principle
(OCP). This is illustrated in (67) and (68). In the first example the /H:L/ word
/ɡáŋá/ 'donkey' modifies the /Hꜜ:Hꜜ/ word /kwéntʼéꜜ/ 'tail', the former
taking its citation melody /H/ and the latter its construct melody /Hꜜ/. The
two H tones merge and the final floating L creates a falling contour on the
utterance-final lengthened vowel. In the second example the /L:L/ word /kùrù/
'genet' modifies the /H:L/ word /tókó/ 'foot', the former taking its citation
melody /L/ and the latter its construct melody /L/. The L tones merge with each
other and the boundary L.
(67)
|
H before a H merges.
|
|
ɡáŋá
|
kʰwénɗê
|
|
→
|
|
|
/ɡáŋá
|
kwéntʼéꜜ/
|
|
donkey
|
tail
|
|
'donkey's tail'
|
(68)
|
L before a L
merges.
|
|
kʰùrù
|
tʰòkʰò
|
|
→ |
|
|
/kùrù
|
tòkò/
|
|
genet
|
foot
|
|
'genet's foot'
|
When a floating L is positioned between two
associated H tones, there is neither a TBU for it to associate with and nor
another L to merge with. Association is blocked and the L remains floating, triggering
downstep. This is illustrated in (69). Here the /Hꜜ:Hꜜ/ word /sásóꜜ/ 'monkey'
modifies the /Hꜜ:Hꜜ/ word /kwéntʼéꜜ/ 'tail', with the floating L of the
citation melody of the former causing downstep and the floating L of the
construct melody of the latter being realized as a final falling contour.
(69)
|
Floating L before a H
triggers downstep.
|
|
sásó
|
ꜜkʰwénɗê
|
|
→
|
|
|
/sásóꜜ
|
kwéntʼéꜜ/
|
|
monkey
|
tail
|
|
'monkey's tail'
|
A downstepped H placed before another H
will merge, but this merged H will still remain downstepped relative to a
preceding H. This is illustrated in (70), where the /HꜜH:Hꜜ/ word /káꜜná/ 'dog'
modifies the /Hꜜ:Hꜜ/ word /kwéntʼéꜜ/ 'tail'. Because the citation melody of
/káꜜná/ has downstep word-internally, it is assumed that underlyingly it has
two H tones with an intervening floating L (as depicted in the autosegmental
diagram).
(70)
|
Downstepped H before a H
merges on lowered register.
|
|
kʰáꜜná
|
kʰwénɗê
|
|
→
|
|
|
/káꜜná
|
kwéntʼéꜜ/
|
|
dog
|
tail
|
|
'dog's tail'
|
Finally, when a floating H is
positioned before an associated L, it will spread to the TBU and delink the L.
This can be seen in (71), where the /Lꜛ:Hꜜ/ word /wàŋàꜛ/ 'chicken' modifies
the /H:L/ word /tókó/ 'foot'. While in most modified contexts the word
/tókó/ would take its construct melody /L/ and surface as [tʰòkʰò], here it
surfaces as [tʰókʰò] because of the intervening H from the citation melody of
/wàŋàꜛ/. This type of delinking only occurs with a floating H and only within
a constructed phrase; an associated H will not spread and delink an associated
L, and a floating H will not cause delinking of a L across a phrasal boundary
(i.e. it will not interfere with a citation melody).
(71)
|
Floating H
before a L delinks L.
|
|
wàŋà
|
tʰókʰò
|
|
→
|
|
|
/wàŋàꜛ
|
tòkò/
|
|
chicken
|
foot
|
|
'chicken's foot'
|
Before moving on to verb tone
melodies I will briefly illustrate the LTCs attested for monosyllabic nouns,
which I subdivide into open and closed syllable profiles. The LTCs found on
these two syllable profiles are a subset of those attested in the disyllabic
nouns (eight of the ten).
Open-syllable monosyllabic
nouns (i.e. CVV and CCVV) attest four of the ten LTCs: /Hꜜ:Hꜜ/, /HꜜH:Hꜜ/, /LHꜜ:L/, /Lꜛ:Hꜜ/. Examples of each of these are given in
Figure 27 below. This subset is quite surprising in terms
of which LTCs are not attested, for example the absence of the simple citation
melodies /H/, /HL/, /L/, and /LH/, and simple construct melody /L/.
LTC
|
Underlying Form
(Citation : Construct)
|
Isolation
(Citation)
|
Plural Nominal Marker
(Citation)
|
Plural Distal Demonstrative
(Construct)
|
/Hꜜ:Hꜜ/
|
/sííꜜ/
|
'wax'
|
síì
|
sííɡù
|
ʔùɡú
sííɡù
|
/HꜜH:Hꜜ/
|
/sáꜜá :
sááꜜ/
|
'woman'
|
sáꜜá
|
sáꜜáɡú
|
ʔùɡú
sááɡù
|
/LHꜜ:L/
|
/bàáꜜ :
bàà/
|
'father'
|
bàâ
|
bàáɡù
|
ʔùɡú
bààɡù
|
/Lꜛ:Hꜜ/
|
/kʼààꜛ :
kʼááꜜ/
|
'new thing'
|
kʼàà
|
kʼààɡú
|
ʔùɡú kʼááɡù
|
Figure 27 - Examples of nominal lexical tone classes on
monosyllabic open-syllable words.
Closed-syllable monosyllabic
nouns (i.e. CVC, CCVC, and CVCC) attest another four of the ten LTCs: /H:L/,
/HL:HL/, /LH:L/, and /L:L/. Examples of these are given in Figure 28 below.
LTC
|
Underlying Form
(Citation : Construct)
|
Isolation
(Citation)
|
Plural Nominal Marker
(Citation)
|
Plural Distal Demonstrative
(Construct)
|
/H:L/
|
|
'kingfisher'
|
ɲjákʰ
|
|
|
/HL:HL/
|
/dóbꜜ/
|
'lion'
|
|
|
|
/LH:L/
|
/màkʼꜛ : màkʼ/
|
'fox'
|
màk
|
màkkʼú
|
ʔùɡú màkkʼù
|
/L:L/
|
/wàr/
|
'robin'
|
wàr
|
wàrɡù
|
ʔùɡú wàrɡù
|
Figure 28 - Examples of
nominal lexical tone classes on monosyllabic closed-syllable words.
Again this is an unexpected
subset, since none of these four LTCs overlap with those attested on the
open-syllable monomorphemes. I would have expected both profiles to have all
the same LTCs, or for one to be a subset of the other. It could be argued that the /Hꜜ:Hꜜ/ and /HL:HL/
LTCs are in fact one in the same, since a /HL/ melody on a single syllable
produces a floating L. My reason for differentiating them here is that the
melody on a the bimoraic CVV profile does not behave like the /HL/
melody on a CVCV profile but instead like the /Hꜜ/. If the melody of /sííꜜ/
were indeed /HL/ I would expect surface tone in the plural nominal marker frame
to be *[síìɡù] with a contour on the double vowel (cf. /pátʼà/ à
[pʰáɗàɡù]). However, the actual surface tone is [sííɡù], suggesting that
the L in the melody is floating and attaches causing a falling tone in
isolation due to utterance-final
lengthening. I have then decided to analyze /dóbꜜ/ as /HL/ for simplicity.
As mentioned in §3.3, the
complexity of the tonal melodies found on these monosyllabic morphemes leads me
to believe that they developed from disyllabic proto forms. Closed-syllable
monosyllabic roots likely developed from final vowel elision (*CVCVàCVC),
and open-syllable from consonant elision (*CVCVàCVV). Given the complementary
distribution of LTCs between the two types of monosyllabic nouns, perhaps one
criterion for whether a CVCV proto-form underwent final vowel elision or
consonant elision had to do with the morpheme's tonal melodies.
For simplex verbs (i.e. disyllabic,
monomorphemic verbs) there are six attested LTCs. There are five citation
melodies— /H/, /HꜜH/, /HL/, /LH/, and /Lꜛ/— and four construct melodies— /L/,
/HL/, /Hꜜ/, /Lꜛ/. Four of the citation melodies pair predictably with one of the
four construct melodies, but the citation melody /HꜜH/ forms two LTCs, pairing
with either /HL/ or /Hꜜ/ for its construct melody. A chart of these LTCs is
given in Figure 29 below.
Verb-LTCs
|
|
/L/
|
/HL/
|
/Hꜜ/
|
/Lꜛ/
|
Citation Melodies
|
/H/
|
/H:L/
|
|
|
|
/HꜜH/
|
|
/HꜜH/HL/
|
/HꜜH:Hꜜ/
|
|
/HL/
|
|
/HL:HL/
|
|
|
/LH/
|
/LH:L/
|
|
|
|
/Lꜛ/
|
|
|
|
/Lꜛ:Lꜛ/
|
Figure 29 -Verbal lexical
tone classes in Ganza based on citation and construct tone melody pairings.
In Figure 30 below I give examples for each of the six
LTCs. The environment chosen to show the citation melody below is the verbal
construction /hàꜛ=ɡa ___=bo/, which is formed from the affirmative marker
/hàꜛ/ the third-person singular subject clitic /=ɡa/ or plural /=ɡu/, and the verbal
clitic /=bo/. The environment chosen for the construct melody is the verbal
construction /hàꜛ=ɡa ___=na/, which is formed the same way only using the
final verbal clitic /=na/. As far as I have been able to ascertain in my
research so far, these two phrases have little if any semantic differences.
LTC
|
Underlying form
(Citation ~ Construct)
|
VC1
(Citation)
|
VC2
(Construct Melody)
|
/H:L/
|
/tákú ~ tàkù/
|
'run.pl'
|
hàɡú tákúbô
|
'they ran'
|
hàɡú
tàkùnà
|
'they ran'
|
/HꜜH:Hꜜ/
|
/ʃíꜜní
~ʃíníꜜ/
|
'work'
|
hàɡá
ʃíꜜníbô
|
'he
works'
|
hàɡá
ʃínínà
|
'he
works'
|
/HꜜH:HL/
|
/wíꜜʃí ~ wíʃì/
|
'send'
|
hàɡá wíꜜʃíbô
|
'he sends'
|
hàɡá wíʃìnà
|
'he sends'
|
/HL:HL/
|
/tájà/
|
'sojourn'
|
hàɡá
tájàbò
|
'he
sojourns'
|
hàɡá
tájànà
|
'he
sojourns'
|
/LH:L/
|
/ɡàrá ~ ɡàrà/
|
'sit.sg'
|
hàɡá ɡàrábô
|
'he sits'
|
hàɡá ɡàrànà
|
'he sits'
|
/Lꜛ:Lꜛ/
|
/pòlòꜛ/
|
'sit.pl'
|
hàɡú pòlòbô
|
'they sit'
|
hàɡú pòlònâ
|
'they sit'
|
Figure 30 - Examples of verbal
lexical tone classes on disyllabic non-allomorphic verbs.
The differing environments for
when a verb stem will take its citation melody and when it will take its
construct melody are given in Figure 31
below. As mentioned before, these environments do not correspond to the
environments for when a verb might take an allomorphic form (cf. Figure 23 in §5.1). This creates a matrix of four
possible verb forms: full-stem with citation melody, full-stem with construct
melody, allomorphic stem with citation melody, and allomorphic stem with
construct melody. Since only a subset of verb roots take an allomorphic form
and since several LTCs have identical citation and construct melodies, while some
verbs have all four variants some have only two and some only one. These
various possibilities are illustrated in Figure 32
with the verb /swáná/ 'to count' having four stem variants, /súꜜnú/ 'to cross'
having two tonal variants but no stem allomorphy, /sápáꜜ/ 'to burn' having two
stem allomorphs but no tonal variants, and /sásáꜜ/ 'to bite' only ever taking
one form.
|
|
|
VC1 /=bo/
|
VC2 /=na/
|
Continuous auxiliary /ɡàráꜜ/+
|
Negative /-ánꜜ/
|
Conditional /-n/
|
Non-final-DS /-l/
|
Imperative /-éʃ/, /-èm/
|
Future /-sa/
|
Serial verb
|
Neg. Imperative /-ánꜜ-éʃ/, /-ánꜜ-èm/
|
Purpose /-sa/
|
Perfect /-ɡwáꜜ/ + vc1/2
|
Jussive /kwámàn/ +
|
Reason /-di/ + /kódò/
|
|
Nominalized /-di/
|
Polar Question
|
|
Non-Final-SS /-p/
|
Relative Clause /-di/
|
|
Content Question /-e/
|
|
|
Figure 31 -Environments
for construct melody in Ganza verbs.
|
|
Four forms
|
Two forms
(tonal)
|
Two forms
(allomorphy)
|
One form
|
Full
Stem
|
/swáná : swànà/
|
/súꜜnú : súnúꜜ/
|
/sápáꜜ/
|
/sásáꜜ/
|
Allomorphic
Stem
|
/swén : swèn/
|
|
/sápꜜ/
|
|
gloss
|
'to count'
|
'to cross over'
|
'to burn'
|
'to bite'
|
Figure 32 – Examples of
verb stem variation resulting from interaction of stem allomorphy and LTCs.
Perhaps the most surprising aspect
of the distribution of the verbal tone melodies shown in Figure 31 above is the "agreement" category,
where dependent forms such as the continuous auxiliaries, non-final different-subject
verbs, and serial verb constructions will take either their citation or their
construct melody depending on the melody of the main verb. This is illustrated
in (72) and (73). These two sentences have essentially the same semantic
content, but the former uses the verbal clitic /=bo/, triggering the use of the
citation melody for the main verb and two preceding serial verbs, and the
latter uses the verbal partical /=na/, triggering the use of the construct
melody in all three verbs.
(72)
|
Serial verbs with citation melody: /LH/ + /HꜜH/ +
/LH/
|
|
tʼúmán
|
pʰáɗàdì
|
pʼòʃó
|
kʰíꜜʔá
|
jèppô
|
|
/tʼúmán
|
pátʼà=di
|
pʼòʃó
|
kíꜜʔá
|
jèpʼꜛ=bo/
|
|
yesterday
|
deer=1sg.sbj
|
chase
|
catch
|
kill=vc1
|
|
(73)
|
Serial verbs with construct melody: /L/ + /HL/
+ /L/
|
|
tʼúmán
|
pʰáɗàdì
|
pʼòʃò
|
kʰíʔà
|
jèpʼnà
|
|
/tʼúmán
|
pátʼà=di
|
pʼòʃò
|
kíʔà
|
jèpʼ=na/
|
|
yesterday
|
deer=1sg.sbj
|
chase
|
catch
|
kill=vc2
|
|
As with the nouns, the verbal
melodies will react differently when placed adjacent to toneless morphemes as
opposed to tone-bearing morphemes, especially in regards to floating tones.
This can be seen, for example, with the addition of the toneless applicative
suffix /-ta/ compared with the perfect marker /ɡwáꜜ/.
When a floating H of a verb
root melody is adjacent to a toneless morpheme it will associate with that
morpheme, as with the /Lꜛ:Lꜛ/ word /tìʔìꜛ/ 'protect' in (74). In this case
there is no spreading of the H to the final morpheme /=na/, but instead the boundary
L associates with it first. When placed before a morpheme with an overt H, like
/-ɡwáꜜ/ in (75), the floating H will merge due to the OCP.
(74)
|
Floating H
associates to toneless morpheme.
|
|
hàɡá
|
tʰìʔìtʰánà
|
|
→
|
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
|
tìʔìꜛ-ta=na/
|
|
aff=3m.sbj
|
protect-apl=vc2
|
|
'he
protects it'
|
(75)
|
Floating H
deletes or merges with following H.
|
|
hàɡá
|
tʰìʔìɡwánà
|
|
→
|
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
|
tìʔìꜛ-ɡwáꜜ=na/
|
|
aff=3m.sbj
|
protect-pfv=vc2
|
|
'he has
protected'
|
A floating L placed before a
toneless morpheme will associate with that TBU, as expected. This can be seen
in (76) with the /HꜜH:Hꜜ/ verb /ʃúꜜká/ 'push', here shown utilizing its
construct melody /Hꜜ/. After associating, adjacent identical tones will merge. However,
a floating L placed before a morpheme with an overt H will cause downstep. This
is shown in (77), when /ʃúkáꜜ/ is positioned before /-ɡwáꜜ/.
(76)
|
Floating L associates with toneless morpheme.
|
|
hàɡá
|
ʃúkʰátʰànà
|
|
→
|
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
|
ʃúkáꜜ-ta=na/
|
|
aff=3m.sbj
|
protect-apl=vc2
|
|
'he pushes
it'
|
(77)
|
Floating L downsteps following H.
|
|
hàɡá
|
ʃúkʰáꜜɡwánà
|
|
→
|
|
|
/hàꜛ=ɡa
|
ʃúkáꜜ-ɡwáꜜ=na/
|
|
aff=3m.sbj protect-pfv=vc2
|
|
'he has pushed'
|
In this paper I have provided a description
and partial analysis of the phonological system of Ganza, a language which was
previously undescribed in any published material. I presented evidence of a
twenty-three consonant phoneme system, including a three way
voiced-ejective-voiceless contrast in the oral stops and sibilants— a well
attested feature of Omotic languages. Also included was evidence of a
nasalizing glottal phoneme /ʔ̃/, which causes
unmotivated (phonemic) nasality on preceding and following vowels and contrasts
word medially with the oral glottal stop /ʔ/. Next I presented evidence for a
five vowel phoneme system that, unlike most Omotic languages, does not utilize
vowel length as a primary contrastive feature. I then looked at the structure
of the word in terms of syllable structure and phonotactics. The syllable
structure of Ganza in particular was shown to be much more liberal than some of
its neighbouring Mao languages, with a maximal syllable of Cw/jVNC. Having laid
the foundations of basic phonemic contrast and word structure, I described two
prominent morphophonemic phenomena of particular relevance. The first was the allomorphy
of noun and verb stems by either FV-Elision or FV-Elision and V-Alternation.
While similar processes are attested in other Omotic languages, particularly
FV-Elision in other Mao languages (Ahland 2009, Girma 2015), Ganza was shown to
be unique in that these only apply to a subset of noun and verb roots in
certain grammatical environments, not ubiquitously. The second phenomenon I
described was the interplay of SV-Elision, voice and manner assimilation, and
final devoicing in the stacking of suffixes and clitics on noun and verb roots.
Here it was shown that the phonology makes a distinction between suffixes and
clitics, and that the application of the aforementioned processes appears to
happen serially, lending support to a serial theory of phonology rather than a simultaneous
one. Finally, I gave an introduction to the tone system. I showed that Ganza
has two tonemes— high (H) and low (L)— as
well as phonemic downstep, and that these have a high functional load in the
language. Next I introduced basic tonal phenomena such as downdrift, construct
melodies, toneless morphemes, rightward unbounded tonal spread, and boundary
tones. Most notable of these is the construct melody, a phenomenon in which a noun
or verb root takes an alternate melody in certain grammatical constructions.
Lastly I described and exemplified the ten lexical tone classes (pairings of
citation and construct melodies) for simplex nouns and the six for simplex
verbs.
This paper was designed as a
first step in the description of Ganza, and more papers are planned from the
data I was able to gather. Much more
research is required on the language, especially in regards to the TAM system,
verbal complex, syntax, and discourse features of the language. A comparative
study with the dialects of Sudanese Ganza would also be beneficial, as
unpublished data sources from those groups seem to indicate some phonological
and syntactic differences. Finally, as Omotic is still a rather young and
understudied language family and Mao the most data-deficient of all its
subfamilies, this newly available data should be considered for comparative
analysis and reconstruction.
1 first person
2 second person
3 third person
acc accusative
aff affirmative
apl applicative
dem demonstrative
dir directional
dist distal
H high tone
imp imperative
irr irrealis
f feminine
FV-elision final vowel elision
fut future
L low tone
LTC lexical tone class
m masculine
med medial
neg negative
nm nominal marker
OCP obligatory contour principle
pfv perfect
pl plural
poss possessive
prox proximal
purp purpose
sbj subject
sg singular
SV-elision suffix vowel elision
TBU tone-bearing unit
vc1 verbal clitic 1 /=bo/,
undetermined semantics
vc2 verbal clitic 2 /=na/,
undetermined semantics
VOT voicing onset time
References
Ahland, Colleen A.
2004. Linguistic Variation within
Gumuz: A Study of the Relationship Between Historical Change and
Intelligibility. Arlington: University of Texas at Arlington PhD
dissertation.
Ahland, Michael B.
2006. Nasal Spreading, Rhinoglottophilia and the Genesis of a Non-Etymological
Nasal Consonant in Mesmes. In Rebecca T. Cover and Yuni Kim (eds.), Proceedings of the thirty-first annual
meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: February 18-20, 2005, 13-24.
Ahland, Michael B.
2009. Aspects of Northern Mao (Bambassi-Diddesa) Phonology. Linguistic Discovery 7(1):1-42.
Ahland, Michael B.
2012. A Grammar of Northern Mao (Màwés
Aasʼè). Eugene: University of Oregon PhD dissertation.
Beachy, Marvin D.
2005. An overview of Central Dizin phonology
and morphology. Arlington: University of Texas at Arlington master's thesis
(unpublished).
Bender, M. Lionel.
1975. Omotic: The New Afroasiatic
Language Family. University Museum Studies 3. Carbondale: Southern Illinois
University.
Bender, M. Lionel.
2000. Comparative morphology of the
Omotic languages. Munich: Lincom Europa.
Bender, M. Lionel.
2003. Omotic lexicon and phonology. (Published
by author)
Bybee, Joan. 2004. Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Crass, Joachim
& Ronny Meyer. 2008. Ethiopia. In Bernd Heine & Derek Nurse (eds.) A Linguistic Geography of Africa. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Debela Goshu
& Girma A. Demeke. 2005. A Grammatical Sketch and some Socio-linguistic
Aspects of a Dying Language, Anfillo. In ELRC
Working Papers 1(1):52-86.
Ford, David. 2013.
Ganza Wordlist. SIL Ethiopia (unpublished).
Fleming, Harold C.
1969. The Classification of West Semitic within Hamito-Semitic. In Daniel
McCall (ed.) Eastern Africa History,
3-27. New York: Praegar.
Fleming, Harold C.
1974. Omotic as an Afroasiatic Family. SAL Supplement
5:81-94.
Fleming, Harold C.
1976. Omotic Overview. In Marvin Lionel Bender (ed.), Non-Semitic Languages of Ethiopia (Monograph no. 5, Occasional
Papers Series, Committee on Ethiopian studies), 299-323. East Lansing: African
Studies Center, Michigan State University.
Getachew Kassa. 2015. A
Grammar of Hoozo. Addis Ababa: Addis Ababa University PhD dissertation.
Girma Mengistu. 2015.
A Grammar of Sezo. Addis Ababa: Addis
Ababa University PhD dissertation.
Hayward, Richard J.
1986. Remarks on Omotic sibilants. In M. Bechhaus-Gerst and F. Serzisko (eds.),
Cushitic-Omotic. Papers from the
International Symposium on Cushitic and Omotic Languages, Cologne. Hamburg:
Helmut Buske Verlag.
Hayward, Richard J.
(ed.). 1990. Omotic Language Studies.
London: School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London.
Hayward, Richard J. 1991. Concerning vocalic alternation in
North Omotic verb paradigms. In Bulletin of the School of Oriental and
African Studies 54(3):535-553. London: University of London.
Hayward, Richard J. 2003. The ‘empty quarter’ of Afroasiatic
Linguistics. In Jacqueline Lecarme (ed.), Research in Afroasiatic Grammar
II: Selected papers from the Fifth Conference on Afroasiatic Languages, Paris, 2000, 241-61. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins
Publishing Company.
Hayward, Richard J. 2009. What's been Happening in Omotic? Journal of Ethiopian Studies
42(1-2):85-106.
Hellenthal, Anne-Christie. 2005. Notes on Mao. Addis Ababa: SIL
Ethiopia (unpublished).
Hellenthal, Anne-Christie. 2010. A Grammar of Sheko. Utrecht: Landelijke Onderzoekschool
Taalwetenschap (Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics).
Hofmeister,
Loriann. 2009. Ganza Statistics. SIM-Sudan (unpublished).
Hofmeister,
Loriann. 2010. A Ganza Language Learning Manual. SIM Sudan (unpublished).
James, Wendy. 1965.
Ganza wordlist (unpublished).
Jordan, Linda,
Hussein Mohammed and Angela Davis. 2004. Survey of Northern Mao, Kwama and Komo
(unpublished).
Joswig, Andreas.
2008. A Brief Grammar of the Borna Language. Addis Ababa: SIL Ethiopia (unpublished).
Kievit,
Dirk & Erika Robertson. 2012. Notes on Gwama Grammar. In Studies in African Linguistics 41(1):39-97.
Kiparsky, Paul
.1982. Word-formation and the Lexicon. . In F Ingemann (ed.), Proceedings of the Mid-America Linguistics
Conference. Lawrence, Kansas.
Kiparsky, Paul.
2000. Opacity and Cyclicity. The Linguistic
Review 17:351-367.
Krell, Amy. 2011. A
Sociolinguistic Survey of the Ganza, Komo, and "Baruun be Magtole"
Language Groups. SIL Electronic
Electronic Survey Report 2011-039.
Küspert,
Klaus-Christian. 2015. The Mao and Komo Languages in the Begi–Tongo area in
Western Ethiopia: Classification, Designations, Distribution. Linguistic Discovery 13(1):1-63.
Leslau, Wolf. 1959. A Dictionary of Moča. Berkley, CA:
University of California Publications in Linguistics.
Lewis, M. Paul (ed.),
2014. Ethnologue: Languages of the World.
Seventeenth edition. Dallas: SIL International. Online version:
http://www.ethnologue.com/
Magnusson,
Carol, Hussein Mohammed and Linda Jordan. 2007. Ganza Survey Proposal. (unpublished).
Matisoff, James A.
1975. Rhinoglottophilia: The mysterious connection between nasality and
glottality. In Charles Ferguson, Larry M. Hyman, and John J. Ohala (eds.), Nasalfest: Papers from a Symposium on Nasals
and Nasalization. Stanford: Language Universals Project, Department of
Linguistics, Stanford University.
Mohammed
Ali & Andrzej Zaborski. 1990. Handbook
of the Oromo Language. Warsaw: Polska Akademia Nauk.
Mulugeta Seyoum.
2008. A Grammar of Dime. Utrecht:
Landelijke Onderzoekschool Taalwetenschap (Netherlands Graduate School of
Linguistics).
Neudorf,
Andreas and Susanne Neudorf. 2007. Bertha-English-Amharic
Dictionary. Addis Ababa: SIL Ethiopia.
Prince, Alan and
Paul Smolensky. 1993. Optimality Theory:
Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. New Brunswick: Rutgers
University.
Reidhead, Paris W.
1947. Note on the Ganza Language:
Preliminary Descriptive Analysis. Melut: SIM Sudan (unpublished).
Snider, Keith. 1999.
The Geometry and Features of Tone.
Arlington: University of Texas at Arlington & the Summer Institute of
Linguistics.
Teshome
Yehualashet. 2006. Remarks on the phonology of Kwama/Komo (Nilo-Saharan). In LISSAN Journal of African Languages &
Linguistics 20(1-2):141-148.
Thiel, Rolf. 2006. Is Omotic Afroasiatic? Proceedings from
the David Dwyer retirement symposium, Michigan State University, East Lansing,
21 October 2006.
Thiel, Rolf. 2012.
Omotic. In Lutz Edzard (ed.), Semitic and
Afroasiatic: Challenges and Opportunities, 369-384. (Porta Linguarum Orientalium, Neue Serie. Bd. 24) Harrassowitz
Verlag.
Zelealem,
[Mollaligne] Leyew. 2005. Gwama: a little
known endangered language of Ethiopia. ELRC (Ethiopian Languages Research
Centre) working papers 1. 1-52.
Joshua Smolders
SIL International
josh_smolders@sil.org