Volume 8 Issue 1 (2010)
DOI:10.1349/PS1.1537-0852.A.372
Note: Linguistic Discovery uses Unicode characters
to represent phonetic symbols. Please see Optimizing Display
for requirements to accurately reproduce this page.
Optimizing Classical Maps
Comment on ‘Analyzing Semantic Maps: A Multifactorial
Approach’ by Andrej L. Malchukov (2010)
Heiko Narrog
Tohoku University
To state my bias as a reader and commentator in the
beginning, I should say that the approach taken by Malchukov is very close to my
own approach to semantic maps, so I am naturally sympathetic towards his
contribution. However, I believe that the breadth of the issues, the wealth of
data and the depth of consideration presented therein make still it a
particularly outstanding paper even if one might not likewise sympathize with
the author’s stance. As I basically share this stance, my comment will
mainly serve to recapitulate his argumentation, and fill in possible gaps where
this seems appropriate.
Malchukov starts out by contrasting the ‘traditional’ or
‘classical’ approach to semantic maps with the
‘alternative’ approach, that which we may also call
‘statistical maps’. He makes clear that both approaches have
advantages and drawbacks. According to Malchukov, the most significant
disadvantages of classical maps are
|
a.
|
their incapability of representing frequency,
|
|
b.
|
the spread of ‘vacuous’ maps if minority patterns are taken
into account on a par with majority patterns.
|
From a certain perspective, one may view (b) as a side
effect of (a), as it also pertains to frequency. Another factor that could be
added here, and which is also related to frequency, at least partially, is that
classical maps are (c) not good at representing degrees of similarity in general
because of the limitations in arranging category labels in a two-dimensional
space. One result of such limitation is that two meanings or functions placed
close to each other may not in fact be connected by a line and may stand in a
relationship of direct meaning extension, while “related” meanings
might have to be placed further apart. In other words, the distance between
meanings or functions is not always indicative of their similarity or
relatedness. In Malchukov’s view, however, representation of similarity is
not the foremost task of a semantic map anyway (cf. section 7). As for their
advantages, according to Malchukov, classical maps can
|
d.
|
capture semantic connections between individual categories,
|
|
e.
|
represent a diachronic dimension (see also Narrog 2010, and van der
Auwera 2008), and
|
|
f.
|
feed directly into semantic analysis.
|
That statistical maps lack these advantages is their major
drawback, while, conversely, their strength lies in the representation of
frequencies, thus reversing (a) and (c) in particular.
Malchukov’s main goal in this paper is to advance on the
difficulty which problem (b) presents for classical maps, and thereby further
establish classical maps as an effective tool in semantic analysis. The task is
to avoid inclusion of minority patterns which are not due to semantic similarity
and thus must be considered as noise when constructing a map. One could go a
step further and claim that the process described by Malchukov could be
profitably applied to the construction of statistically plotted maps as well,
provided that these maps are not constructed mechanically on the basis of a set
of data, but the data (e.g. translation data such as those used by Wälchli
2010) are first analyzed by hand. After all, (b), depending on the point of
view, may be perceived as a problem common to the statistically-driven approach
as well. Malchukov claims that the following types of “noise” can be
identified (the labeling I attach to these types is not identical to
Malchukov’s but reflects my own understanding).
- markedness effects, specifically neutralization of
semantic/functional contrasts in the marked category of a paradigm (section
2)
- zero marking (section 3)
- polysemies which should not occur in natural language data for
reasons of distinguishability, but occasionally do occur due to coincidences in
diachronic development (section 4)
- unusual polysemies of lexical items involved in grammatical patterns
(section 5)
- wide-ranging polysemies of lexical verbs when they begin to
grammaticalize as serial verbs (section 6.1)
- polysemies due to reanalysis rather than meaning extension (section
6.2)
- lack of linking meaning/function due to gram replacement (section
6.3)
As far as I know, Malchukov is the first to present all
these interfering factors together in one paper. “Noise type” (7)
was already brought up by Haspelmath (2003:236), but I was unaware of many
others, and I assume other readers were too, at least in this context.
Empirically speaking (as is obvious from Malchukov’s description),
these problems are hardly likely to occur all together in a single area of
grammar on one map, but depend on the type of categories involved. If a map of
grammatical categories needs to include the features of lexical items (problems
4 and 5), it is natural that idiosyncratic features of the lexical items should
be excluded from the grammatical map to the extent possible. Labeling factors
(1), (2), (6), and (7) as ‘interfering’ should also be
uncontroversial. Especially (7) is already well established, and it is highly
problematic to speak of a polysemy or meaning extension of zero marking (2). The
least obvious “noise” in my view is (3), namely, those polysemies
that should not occur for reasons of distinguishability but nevertheless do, in
some rare cases. These cases, as presented by Malchukov, are the quirky product
of regular semantic extension, and thus fulfill the criterion for being
represented in a semantic map. Semantic maps should represent empirically
attested meaning extensions as fully as possible, even if they turn out to be
inconvenient for the theory and result in inelegant hypotheses on semantic
similarity and meaning extension. This is a point to which Malchukov would
certainly also agree. I believe that, for classical maps, one possibility to
represent such quirky or rare connections would be to choose different types of
lines.
One type of noise which is fairly well-known but not mentioned by
Malchukov is case syncretism due to phonological changes (primarily phonological
reduction of the desinences, cf. e.g. Baerman 2010, section 14.4). Here, the motivation for formal identity is also non-semantic, and
excluding the resultant polysemies from semantic maps seems to be sensible. A
more general question related to this “noise” might be to what
extent inflectional paradigms with phonologically heavily reduced desinences can
still provide a reliable base for a study that is grounded in iconicity between
(phonological) form and meaning.
References
Baerman, Matthew. 2010. Case syncretism. The Oxford handbook of
case, ed. by Andrej Malchukov and Andrew Spencer, 219-230. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Haspelmath, Martin. 2003. The geometry of grammatical meaning:
Semantic maps and cross-linguistic comparison. The new psychology of language,
ed. by Michael Tomasello, vol. 2, 211-243. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Malchukov, Andrej L. 2010. Analyzing semantic maps: A
multifactorial approach. Linguistic Discovery, this issue. doi:10.1349/ps1.1537-0852.a.350
Narrog, Heiko. 2010. A diachronic dimension in maps of case
functions. Linguistic Discovery, this issue. doi:10.1349/ps1.1537-0852.a.352
van der Auwera, Johan 2008. In defense of classical semantic maps.
Theoretical Linguistics 34/1.39-46. doi:10.1515/thli.2008.002
Wälchli, Bernhard. 2010. Similarity semantics and building
probabilistic semantic maps from parallel texts. Linguistic Discovery, this
issue. doi:10.1349/ps1.1537-0852.a.356
Author’s contact information:
Heiko Narrog
Graduate School of Information Sciences
Tohoku University
Kawauchi 41
Aoba-ku
Sendai-shi, 980-8576
Japan
narrog@gmail.com
|