Volume 8 Issue 1 (2010)
DOI:10.1349/PS1.1537-0852.A.371
Note: Linguistic Discovery uses Unicode characters
to represent phonetic symbols. Please see Optimizing Display
for requirements to accurately reproduce this page.
Connectivity in Implicational Maps
Authors’ reply to ‘The Added Value of the
Connectivity Hypothesis for the Map of Parts of Speech’ by Caterina Mauri
(2010)
Kees Hengeveld & Eva van Lier
University of Amsterdam & Lancaster University
Mauri (2010), in her response to our paper (Hengeveld and
van Lier 2010), states that adding the
connectivity constraint to the
sets of constraints we propose in our implicational map of parts of speech would
increase the accuracy of our model.
Mauri’s point is well taken. The connectivity constraint has
strong empirical support in a wide variety of semantic domains, and indeed, if
this constraint is added to the ones we propose, we can exclude two systems that
were predicted by our original model but have not been attested. Thus, with the
connectivity constraint added, the model would predict 15 possible systems, 13
of which are attested either in their ‘pure’ form or in combination
with another predicted system.
This leaves us with two predicted but as yet unattested systems. These
remaining systems are given in Figures 19 and 22 in our contribution to this
volume, and are repeated below for convenience.
The system in Figure 19 is perhaps just as plausible as a system which
has verbs only (represented in Figure 14 of our contribution to this volume). In
languages of both these types, lexemes are used for predication, while reference
is established indirectly, i.e. through the predication of properties and
relations in which a referent is typically involved.
Figure 19.
The system in Figure 22 is arguably just as plausible as a
system which lacks modifiers within predicate phrases only (represented in
Figure 21 of our contribution to this issue). We presented Tagalog as an example
of a language of the latter type, the reason being that this flexible language
has no slot for modifiers within predicate phrases. One could imagine that the
opposite situation, a flexible language which lacks a slot for modifiers within
referential phrases, could also exist, and would manifest itself as strongly
appositional in nature.
Figure 22.
Thus, in all, adding the connectivity constraint would
improve the predictive power of our implicational map, and would leave us with
15 predicted systems, 13 (pure or mixed) attested systems, and 2 unattested
systems.
Our hesitation to take this step has to do with the fact that our
approach to implicational maps is hierarchical in nature: hierarchies themselves
are subject to higher hierarchies, and are ordered in their application. The
question is then what counts as contiguous in such an approach. It is evident
that contiguity applies along the predication-reference parameter and along the
head-modifier parameter within each of the domains of predication and reference,
but no real predictions can be made concerning the cases that cross-cut these
two parameters. And since we have two parameters only and each of our parameters
has only two values, the contiguity constraint then becomes vacuous.
A real test case for the issue at hand is one in which more than two
parameters with more than two values each would be combined into an
implicational map, or rather, into an implicational multidimensional space. In
such a case, contiguity has to be redefined in terms of connections rather than
strict two-dimensional contiguity. Our case does not permit us to make any
strong claims in this area. We would thus be happy to adopt Mauri’s
valuable suggestion, but only after further experimenting with the notion of
connectivity in implicational maps.
References
Hengeveld, Kees and Eva van Lier. 2010. An implicational map of
parts of speech. Linguistic Discovery, this issue. doi:10.1349/ps1.1537-0852.a.348
Mauri, Caterina. 2010. The added value of the Connectivity
Hypothesis for the map of parts of speech. Comment on Hengeveld and van Lier
2010. Linguistic Discovery, this issue. doi:10.1349/ps1.1537-0852.a.370
Author’s contact information:
Kees Hengeveld
Department of Theoretical Linguistics
University of Amsterdam
Spuistraat 210
1012 VT Amsterdam
The Netherlands
p.c.hengeveld@uva.nl
Eva van Lier
Department of Linguistics and English Language
Lancaster University
LA1 4YT Lancaster
United Kingdom
e.vanlier@lancaster.ac.uk
|