Volume 1 Issue 1 (2002)
DOI:10.1349/PS1.1537-0852.A.101
Note: Linguistic Discovery uses Unicode characters
to represent phonetic symbols. Please see Optimizing Display
for requirements to accurately reproduce this page.
What Does Digital Technology Have to Do with Yaghan?
Lenore A. Grenoble and Lindsay J. Whaley
Dartmouth College
1. Introduction
Research that involves the collection of language data
is notoriously labor intensive, often requiring fieldwork in remote places,
where the travel alone is extremely time-consuming, not to mention expensive.
Funding and institutional support for such research is limited, and often
impossible to obtain without the attendant claim on the part of the researcher
that paradigm-shifting theoretical issues lurk behind the data. Prestige within
the academy does not come through careful analyses of the grammatical categories
of Oneida, the nasal phonemes of Tequistlatec, or vowel mergers in Pittsburgh,
but through pronouncements of innate linguistic structures, the evolutionary
development of language or breakdowns in communication between the sexes.
Such realities provide powerful incentives to avoid constructing
research projects that entail too great an emphasis on the gathering of primary
linguistic data. It is a wonder, then, that so many linguists still do so. And
yet most of these data—dutifully recorded in notebooks, computer files, as
well as on cassette and video tapes—are hidden away in offices and library
collections around the world. Most of this information will never find its way
out of these caches. Collectively, linguists know far more about the languages
of the world than one might gather from published material, but that knowledge
remains disconnected and impossible to access.
Why should this be? Part of the reason, it must be admitted, is
the understandable propensity of the scholar to hoard pure data until she has
time to analyze the information herself and glean what theoretical wheat may be
there. Just as significant, the linguist shelves his fieldnotes because of the
failure of tenure and promotion committees to recognize the importance of data
collection; the publication of lexicons, for example, is often not recognized as
a scholarly contribution, but as something of a service
activity.[1] In addition to these two,
an equally noteworthy, if not more significant, reason for the failure to make
field data available is that there are few convenient ways to disseminate this
information. We will highlight the ramifications of this truism by telling the
tale of Yaghan and Ona, languages about which little is now known despite the
fact that they might have ranked as two of the better-studied languages of South
America around the turn of the 20th century, indeed as two of the few that had
audio recordings made at that time.
Before moving to the case of Yaghan and Ona, however, it is
necessary to consider why the dissemination of large quantities of collected
data has never become the routine practice of linguists and anthropologists.
There is not great mystery surrounding the issue. The primary venue to share
information has been paper and ink, but printed materials are, after all,
expensive to create and hard to distribute. Increasingly, in book and journal
form, their raison d’être is established foremost by the
potential for profit, so there are few publishers interested in creating
dictionaries, text collections, grammars, or other pieces of scholarship with a
large descriptive component. This is especially true for minority indigenous
languages, where the perceived readership of such materials is so small as to
preclude their publication.
But this state of affairs need not be. The advent of digital
technology and growing global access to the world wide web has the potential of
radically changing the way that we store and access information on languages,
and thus of how we linguists and anthropologists think about the data we
collect. While the new technologies do not remove the investment of human labor
from data gathering and organization, they facilitate the recording of
high-quality audio clips and video images that can be shared with relative ease.
Such data, in digital form, can usually be manipulated more effectively than
older (tape) formats. Transcribed data are arranged and re-arranged as a matter
of course and can be compactly gathered in computer files. While a plastic CD
may lack the romance of the yellowed pages of fifty field notebooks, we suspect
few linguists would deny the advantages of the former over the latter,
particularly when it comes to making information about language available to an
assortment of people all over the world.
Many of the practical constraints of print media are lifted in the
digital age. Data can now be displayed inexpensively for billions of people
since the technological infrastructure is largely in place. The time-lags of
traditional publications are greatly reduced. Most importantly, the information
is not restricted to static graphic images. As many a web site demonstrates
already, audio and video data have become a regular part of the transmission of
language data.
Still, the potential of the digital age to transform the field of
linguistics remains just that—a potential. As just one example, current
electronic journals tend to be digitized forms of printed versions, and
scholarly articles therein continue to be written in a traditional way, not
making use of the possibilities of multimedia. In our opinion, this is a serious
mistake given the dynamic nature of language coupled with the imminent loss of
so many of our planet’s languages. Consider the value of word lists and
dictionaries in understanding the linguistic scene of the New World in 1850. Now
imagine how much more valuable this information would be if it had an audio and
video dimension. The significance of sound and audio data for our own generation
and for generations to come cannot be overstated.
We now attempt to give a human face to what has been written here
as an abstraction, by presenting a brief study on the collection and storage of
language data in a past era. It is the story of two languages that are extinct,
or very close to it. Despite over a century of interest in the languages, we do
not know enough about them to classify them, to make a definitive statement
about their phonology, or to say much of anything about how clauses are
constructed in these languages. What we do know about them is thanks to some
fairly extraordinary efforts on the part of a missionary, a priest, an
ethnographer and an explorer. But the technology of their times has left
relatively little for us now, putting the possibilities of the digital age for
us in relief.
2. The Furlong
Collection
Social trends during the period of European colonization
resulted in the documentation and description of many languages around the
world. The most obvious and well-known to linguists stemmed from the extensive
missionary efforts that were undertaken at this time. Then—as
today—many of these missionaries and clergy remained with the local
populations for years at a time, learning their language(s) and often becoming
community members, to varying degrees. The activities of the waves of
missionaries can be judged from a number of vantage points, but regardless, one
legacy of this group of people is that we have documentation about scores of the
world’s languages. Crucially, many of the missionaries were among the
first Westerners/Europeans to interact with indigenous groups in other parts of
the world, and their written records provide critical data for these indigenous
languages prior to contact with an Indo-European language.
A second, less recognized source of linguistic information comes
from the explorers of the time. An example is provided by Charles Wellington
Furlong, who left behind a healthy amount of personal papers, notebooks,
photographs and memorabilia from his adventures. Much of this is housed in our
own institution’s special collections, the Rauner Library. Furlong joined
the Explorer’s Club in late 1800’s and traveled to a number of
remote places in Africa and South America, most importantly for present purposes
to Tierra del Fuego in 1907–08. Here, at the “ends of the
earth,” he became fascinated with the tribes of the region and made
recordings of two of the languages spoken there at that time. These are
Yaghan, more commonly called Yamana now, and Ona (or
Selknam / Selk’nam). Although Furlong also investigated a
third ethnic group, whom he called the Alaculoof (more currently known as
the Qawasqar), there are no audio recordings of this language in our
holdings.
Furlong took with him an Edison phonograph with wax cylinders. The
recording equipment and cylinders weighed approximately forty pounds, and so, as
he himself indicates in his personal papers, the decision to take it on the
expedition was itself not trivial. He recognized the importance of the
recordings made on this expedition, and some years later assisted in having the
wax-cylinder recordings transferred to reel-to-reel tapes, also to be found
currently in the Rauner Library at Dartmouth and in the Library of Congress. The
significance of such recordings for our time is magnified by the status of the
languages with which Furlong had contact, all of which are now either extinct or
nearly so.
3. The Fuegian Languages
of Furlong's Expedition
Furlong was in Tierra del Fuego in 1907-08. He found
what he identified as four distinct ethno-linguistic groups living there, which
he grouped together in two larger groups: the Alaculoof and the
Yaghan, or “canoe-people,” and the Haush and
Ona, or “foot-people.” The actual genetic relationship of the
languages of these groups is disputed; Grimes (2000) cites Yaghan (or Yamana) as
an isolate, but notes that Tovar (1961) considers it to be closest to Qawasqar
(or what Furlong called Alaculoof) and that it has some relationship to Ona.
Gusinde (1926) recognizes only three languages as being spoken in Tierra del
Fuego at the time of his travels: Halakwulup, Yamana and Selk’nam
(Qawasqar/Alaculoof, Yaghan and Ona). Included among Gusinde’s own papers
on Ona is a three-page manuscript which outlines the distinctive features of
Haush. Based on an examination of these materials, Klakowski (1998:198)
concludes that Gusinde himself was uncertain of the status of Haush. Furlong
(1917) asserts that Haush is not intelligible to either Ona or Yaghan speakers,
while Bridges (1933) classifies a number of “Haush” words as
“Eastern Ona.” Certainly, the six lexical items which Furlong
provides as evidence of the difference between Haush and Ona are not compelling
proof, especially since one is identical (show’k’un
‘three’) and two others conceivably cognate. No evidence is
presented to support or disprove that these could be loanwords. Borrero (1983,
1994) argues that, in light of the overall consistency of both Ona and Haush
material culture, together with the absence of definitive evidence to the
contrary, Haush is best treated as a specialized branch of Ona.
The question of the genetic relationships among these languages
will probably never be answered satisfactorily since the kinds of linguistic
evidence needed to provide proof one way or another are gone. In 1912 Furlong
was told that there were five Haush people remaining on Tierra del Fuego, but he
himself had found only two in 1908. He suggests that the total of five may well
have included the wives of the two Haush men, who would have been counted as
Haush after their marriage according to tribal customs, but had married into the
Haush from different groups. By the time of Furlong’s 1967 manuscript, he
maintains that the “Fuegian” languages were already extinct.
The current vitality of Fuegian languages is somewhat difficult to
assess, but if the languages are not now extinct, they are nearly so. (Although
we note a revitalization program for Yaghan is reported to be in place; see
Aguilera n.d.) By 1924–25, each group had a very small population, with
the Qawasqar the largest group: Qawasqar 150, Yaghan 40-50, Ona 60–70, and
Haush only two or three people (Lothrop 1928:25). Of course, these are
population estimates, not speaker counts, so the vitality of the languages
cannot be determined from the numbers. Salas and Valencia (1990) report that at
least four Yaghan speakers were known to be living in the 1980’s, all
women married to Chilean Spanish speakers. A report from 1997 asserts that only
three Yaghan speakers remained at that time (Reverand Ben Garrett, cited in
Klafkowski 1998:210). Ona is presumed extinct, although Grimes (2000) reports it
as “nearly extinct,” with no population estimates available.
Furlong was not the first Western explorer to reach Tierra del
Fuego. He himself maintained that only Charles Darwin preceded him, but in fact
European contact was initially established in 1580 by a group of Spanish
explorers led by Pedro Sarmiento de Gamboa (Borrero 1994:252). The island itself
had been discovered some sixty years earlier by Ferdinand Magellan, who named it
Tierra del Fuego ‘Land of Fire’ due to the plumes of smoke which
members of his expedition observed. Magellan, however, is not known to have
interacted with the inhabitants of the island.
Historically, the two leaders in research on the languages of
Tierra del Fuego are Reverend Thomas Bridges (1842-1898) and Martin Gusinde SVD
(1886-1969). Although Bridges was not a linguist, his extended time with the
Yaghan (or Yamana, as he called them) made him proficient in their language. He
compiled a large Yaghan (Yamana) dictionary, which to date is the most
comprehensive lexicon of the language ever published, as far as we know.
Given the significance of this piece of work, it is of great
interest that its publication was a precarious endeavor. Bridges had originally
arranged to give the manuscript of the dictionary to the Antarctic explorer
Frederick Cook for publication, but Bridges died before this took place. Lucas
Bridges, son of Thomas, eventually did get the manuscript to Cook under the
agreement that the dictionary would be under his father’s name and the
manuscript returned within one and a half years.
Eleven years later, the dictionary had still not been printed,
nor the manuscript returned. Worse, Cook apparently intended to publish the
dictionary under his own name. At the direct request of Lucas Bridges, Furlong
investigated the status of the manuscript when he returned to the United States
from Tierra del Fuego in 1908. He learned, among other things, that the
manuscript had landed in Gusinde’s hands, who was charged with translating
the Ellis phonetic system (used by Bridges) into a more widely employed phonetic
script. Ultimately, at expense to the Bridges family, a limited edition (300
copies) of the dictionary was published by the Anthropos Institut in
Mödling in 1933. An annotation to the dictionary, written by Furlong,
describes the reasons for the publication delays in greater detail.
Martin Gusinde was a priest of the Catholic Order of the Divine
Word. In 1918 he set off for his first mission to Tierra del Fuego. At the time
he arrived, he found a total of approximately 80 Halakwulup and a depressingly
small number of Ona/Selk’nam, living in two communities. The one, near Rio
del Fuego, consisted of 27 families of 216 individuals, and the other, near Lago
del Fuego, of only five families of 32 individuals. By the time of Anne
Chapman’s missions to Tierra del Fuego (1964-65) she found only six
remaining Ona/Selk’nam (see Guyot 1968:20, fn. 3). Of these six, only one
(Lola Kiepjan) was old enough to have ever lived the traditional lifestyle
(“la vie sauvage”), and was estimated to be 92 years of age then.
She subsequently died in October 1966. To the best of our knowledge, the
Ona/Selk’nam language is now extinct.
The lack of linguistic data in Gusinde’s own publications is
remarkable, considering that Gusinde himself was fairly knowledgeable in all
Fuegian languages.[2]
Klakowski’s research into his archives suggests that Gusinde had intended
to publish at least two separate linguistic studies on the grammar of Ona and on
Alakuluf/Halakwulup, plans which he abandoned around 1930. In contrast to
Gusinde’s archival materials, as described by Klakowski (1998),
Furlong’s collection seems rather haphazard. While Gusinde collected
extensive word lists and made careful notes about phonology, morphology and
syntax, Furlong did not.
4. The Languages of
Tierra del Fuego
Having briefly described some of the early fieldwork carried
out in Tierra del Fuego, we now move to an even briefer description of what is
known about these languages. The term Fuegian languages is used to refer
to a number of languages indigenous to Patagonia, Tierra del Fuego and its
adjacent islands. This is a geographic, not genetic, group which includes
Tehuelche, Puelche and the languages/dialects of Tierra del Fuego itself:
Selk’nam, Haush, Yaghan, and Qawasqar. Some of these are generally
classified as genetically related, while others are not. For example,
Selk’nam (Ona) and Tehuelche are Chon languages; Haush is often considered
to be a dialect of Selk’nam. Each language has a number of variant names.
For example, Puelche (an isolate) is also known as Gennakan, Pampa and Northern
Tehuelche according to Grimes (2000), and Gunua-Kena/Gununa-Kena
is given as an alternate name for Tehuelche, while Clairis (1985) cites
Gennakan and Pampa as alternate names for one single language.
Language names used here follow Grimes (2000). Some dialect distinctions have
been identified within individual languages; Yaghan is reported to have had five
dialects (Koppers 1927); an overview of some classification issues is found in
Key (1978, 1981).
As this suggests, classification is extremely complicated and is
in many ways obfuscated by the many alternate names used for the different
languages. The classification problems are well described by Clairis
(1985:15):
(1) There has often been a tacit assumption that ethnic sub-groups
can be assumed to speak different languages;
(2) Different explorers/researchers who had contact with these
groups have referred to them in different ways, such as the use of the word
‘man’ Yamana or the toponym Yaghan for one and the
same group;
(3) When a group was given a different name by neighboring ethnic
groups it has often been assumed, for lack of any better position, that these
differing names refer, in actuality, to differing languages and ethnic
groups;
(4) What are sometimes deemed to be language/dialect differences
can often be accounted for in terms of the different notation systems used by
various researchers, which in part stem from their own differing languages;
(5) The nomadic lifestyle of these different groups meant that
they had extensive contact with one another, increasing the likelihood of
contact phenomena, such as lexical and phonological borrowings.
The problems in identification and classification thus
stem from a number of problems. One of these is the implicit assumption that the
ethnonyms of given groups, or even more oddly the names given them by outsiders,
reflect a kind of ethnolinguistic reality that can serve as the basis for
genetic classification. This fails to take into account that many subgroups of
larger ethnic groups identify themselves according to clan systems, or by place
names, and so on. Moreover, some of the confusion reported by Clairis can be
traced to a confusion of methodologies (such as the use of different phonetic
scripts) and a lack of solid linguistic training. Earlier researchers did have
access to these groups at a time when the languages were still spoken by
communities of speakers, and yet one result of their inattention to the naming
issues, or technical details, is incomplete information about each of these
languages.
Issues of genetic affiliation could only be answered by in-depth
linguistic analysis. These questions are of special interest with relation to
the groups at hand: Yaghan is the southernmost human language, and solid
linguistic evidence could do much to inform our understanding of New World
migrations. Most of this research remains to be done, and may well never be due
to insufficient evidence. The bulk of existing work focuses on issues of
phonetics and phonology, often based on the study of word lists. The case of
Fuegian languages is so particularly compelling in that they are all seriously
endangered or extinct.[3]
4.1 An Example of
the Problem: The phonology of Yaghan/Yamana
The absence of systematic descriptive work on Yaghan,
particularly on morphology and syntax, is really quite striking when one
considers that fieldwork has been carried out on the language for 120
years.[4] Early studies of Yaghan
include Bridges (1893, 1933) and Gusinde (1926); Bridges’ work served as
the foundation of Hestermann-Hamburg’s (1929) study of Yaghan pronouns;
more recent work includes Guerra (1989, 1992), Poblete Mendoza and Salas (1999),
Salas and Poblete Mendoza (1997), and Salas and Valencia (1990); Aguilera F
(n.d.) provides a sketch of the phonology in his discussion of the development
of an orthography as part of ongoing efforts at Yaghan revitalization.
In these works, the phonological structure of Yaghan has been
studied more thoroughly than any other aspect of the language, and it is in this
realm one might expect to find a reliable depiction of the language.
Unfortunately, even the phonemes of the language have been described variably.
Most research suggests that the language has seven vowel phonemes
(Poblete Mendoza and Salas 1999:108; Salas and Valencia 1990:149):
|
front
|
central
|
back
|
high
|
i
|
|
u
|
mid
|
e
|
ɘ
|
o
|
low
|
æ
|
a
|
|
There is disagreement, however, in the literature as to
the consonant phonemes. Perhaps some of the differences in phonemic inventories
may be attributed to dialect differences, as up to five different dialects have
been claimed to exist (Campbell 1997; Guyot 1968), but this is impossible to
establish.
Salas and Valencia (1990) present the following system of 17
phonemes with, notably, only voiceless stops, affricates and fricatives:
|
bilabial
|
labio-dental
|
alveolar
|
alveo-palatal
|
retroflex
|
velar
|
glottal
|
stop
|
p
|
|
t
|
|
|
k
|
ʔ
|
nasal
|
m
|
|
n
|
|
|
|
|
tap or flap
|
|
|
ɾ
|
|
|
|
|
fricative
|
|
f
|
s
|
ʃ
|
|
|
h
|
affricative
|
|
|
|
tʃ
|
|
|
|
approximant
|
|
|
|
j
|
ɻ
|
w
|
|
lateral
|
|
|
l
|
|
|
|
|
The existence of a glottal fricative /h/ is one
discrepancy; a number of other works cite the existence of a velar fricative /x/
and no glottal fricative (Aguilera F. n.d., Poblete Mendoza and Salas 1999).
Gusinde (1893:1021–2) cites the existence of both a laryngeal /h/ and a
velar /x/. The /h/ occurs only in initial position and is often lost when it
occurs in word combinations that place it in post-vocalic position. He
further notes that Bridges transcribed this sound differently: as an acute
accent in his translation of the Gospel of Luke, and as the letter h in
his own manuscript. This could be interpreted in a variety of ways, but quite
possibly, the /h/ was in a state of flux at the time when Bridges and Gusinde
had contact with the Yaghan, and has been subsequently lost.
Another, relatively minor, issue is the distribution of phonemes:
Salas and Valencia (1990:152, fn 6) assert that in isolated words, vowels in
initial-position can be pronounced with a prolonged onset which can be heard as
a lenis pronunciation of a glottal stop. They do not see this as phonemic.
Rather, it is a coarticulation process that is associated with the initial
vowel; they state elsewhere (p. 155) that the glottal stop cannot occur
word-initially. Furthermore, they cite individual variation among speakers as to
whether it occurs at all. In contrast, Poblete Mendoza and Salas (1999:109)
assert that all consonants in Yaghan occur word-initially; their example of the
glottal stop is ʔa
-nan ‘canoe’.
Even more curious is Gusinde’s (1893) description of Yaghan
sound system which includes a range of voiced consonants. (Salas and Valencia
1990:152 are very clear on this point: “Stops, affricates and fricatives
are always voiceless.” All modern descriptions cite these as only
voiceless, in all positions. Gusinde (1893:1022), however, describes the sound
system in terms of the voiced pairs: /p, b, t, d, s, z, k, g/. Gusinde does not
provide minimal pairs for these consonants, but does show them in contrastive
environments. Some examples are:[5]
Ellis system of transcription which he used, are:
pátux̥
|
wide
|
belaka
|
rain
|
palaala
|
stranger
|
ʃaabaɡuta
|
be content
|
lekkex
|
night
|
taɡu
|
give
|
aki
|
hit
|
ɡeiɡuʃ
|
seagull
|
teska
|
laugh
|
datu
|
walk
|
asikata
|
dolphin
|
aizex
|
strong
|
Gusinde’s description is open to a number of
interpretations. It could be that he was describing a dialect that has since
been lost, or one that at least differs from those described by current
linguists. Alternatively, the voiced consonants could have been lost, possibly
due to language attrition, language change or some other factor. Given the
careful phonetic description that Gusinde provides, we find it to be unlikely
that his analysis is simply flawed. Gusinde—who also relied on
Bridges’ own work—clearly paid close attention to phonetic
detail.
The possible existence of a voicing distinction is just one of the
intriguing, but unanswerable, issues surrounding Yaghan. Given that phonemic
descriptions tend to be among the very first aspects of language analysis for
fieldworkers, the uncertainty found in this realm of Yaghan grammar highlights
that the mere collection of language data is not so valuable as a systematic
collection of data and—crucially—the dissemination of the
information. This, of course, is what Charles Furlong had in mind when he filled
notebooks with observations about Yaghan and Ona and when he decided to haul
forty odd pounds of recording equipment with him on his journey to Tierra del
Fuego. So what went wrong?
5. The Legacy of C. W. Furlong
Charles W. Furlong arrived on Tierra del Fuego at a time
when the Ona and Yaghan communities were still fully vital, as a member of an
explorer’s club, whose goal was basic discovery of new lands and peoples.
Yet Furlong’s interest in Tierra del Fuego was more than passing, and he
did collect materials with every intention of bringing his discoveries to public
attention through publications. There was an self-professed urgency to his work
as he recognized the imminent danger to the vitality of the people and languages
of Tierra del Fuego; his 1917 article ends with a plea to the
“magnanimity” of the governments of Chile and Argentina to intervene
in the situation which was causing the erosion of Ona land and culture,
asserting that “thus could Christianity, spiced with justice and common
sense, be meted out to a splendid aboriginal tribe” (1917:444).
Recognizing the importance of his contact with these people,
Furlong generated copious fieldnotes and a number of sound recordings and during
his stay on Tierra del Fuego. Crucially, he had access to native speaker
communities, where Yaghan and Ona were the community languages, spoken as a
first language by a primarily monolingual population. From this standpoint,
Furlong’s work should be invaluable, particularly since oral recordings of
these languages in a vital stage are now impossible to create: the few known
speakers of Yaghan no longer constitute a speech community, but live separately
from one another in a Spanish-speaking environment, and Ona is no longer spoken
at all.
The Furlong sound archives total several hours of audio recordings,
consisting primarily of songs, some forest calls, and a few snippets of speech.
Of the latter, one of the most audible is a translation of the Lord’s
Prayer into Yaghan, unfortunately read by a non-native speaker (Bridges, presumably),
with intonation that is remarkably reminiscent of Church Latin. The remaining
speech recordings are short, nearly inaudible, and have not been transcribed.
An excerpt has been digitized and is available here:
Excerpt from the recordings
The song recordings are in many ways even less helpful: the
acoustic distortions are much greater and, without any kind of transcription, it
is impossible to determine which segments are language and which not. Despite
Furlong’s good intentions, he ultimately was foiled by the state of the
technology of his time.
Equally regrettable is the fact that Furlong lacked the technical
training necessary for reliable linguistic analysis. Consider some of his
comments regarding Ona: “The Ona language is indescribably guttural and
unmusical, full of clicks, and may be likened to a man clearing his throat
meantime breaking an egg-shell or two in his mouth. These clicks are similar to
the Zulu” (Furlong 1917:439). The presence of clicks in a language
indigenous to Tierra del Fuego would indeed be a significant finding, but should
undoubtedly be viewed with extreme skepticism. One can only regret that Furlong
had no training in phonetics, and that his recordings do not in any way
illustrate these sounds.
Some of Furlong’s analyses did find their way into print.
The vast majority did not, perhaps with good reason given his lack of linguistic
knowledge, but no doubt also because Furlong never ventured beyond a descriptive
account of the languages. One of the more intriguing items in the Furlong
archives is a brief (three pages, double-spaced) manuscript written by Furlong
entitled “Fuegian Linguistics.” The paper provides a sketch of the
lexicon, phonology and morphology of the languages that Furlong studied. It is
accompanied in the archives by a short rejection letter from the editor of
Language, William Bright, dated February 19, 1967. Bright’s note is
succinct and to the point:
“Thank you for bringing your very interesting
Fuegian researches to my attention. I am sorry to say, however, that your paper
does not seem to me suitable for LANGUAGE, which is in general devoted to much
more technical articles. Your manuscript is returned herewith.”
6. Conclusion
Charles Wellington Furlong set out for Tierra del Fuego
in a radically different time, with markedly different goals from those of
modern linguists. Almost 100 years have passed since Furlong’s first
expedition to Tierra del Fuego. From our current standpoint, the impact of his
work is minor, and the materials he has left behind are frustratingly
incomplete. While he recognized the value of the languages and cultures he
encountered and did his best to record them for posterity, the technology he
used, along with his lack of focused methodology, meant that there is little of
value for analysis.
One of the basic issues opened by this case study is the use of
technology for preserving and distributing linguistic data. Clearly, if Furlong
had had access to better technology, the recordings he has left us would be
solve some rather basic questions about the languages of Tierra del Feugo. We
live in a time where the technology does exist, but continues to be
underutilized.
This is not to say that there are not many significant issues
surrounding the use of technology in the field. For the immediate future, at
least, we simply do not know how technology will develop, and the possibility
that digitized data will become obsolete is real. In the case of sound
recordings, there are important questions about the effect of digital formats on
phonetic detail. for all forms of digital data, there are questions of long-term
degradation.
However, our work with the Furlong materials has driven the
following point home: We are currently at a critical moment in the history of
linguistics with the imminent and permanent loss of hundreds of languages.
Though not perfect, the technology at our disposal opens up possibilities
unimaginable to previous generations of fieldworkers and linguists. The creative
exploitation of this technology, maximizing its effectiveness and usability,
depends on developing a culture within our field that expects both established
and upcoming linguists to “become digital.” The field would
certainly be better for it.
References
Aguilera F., Oscar E. (no date) En torno a la estructura
fonológico del yagan. Fonología de la palabra. (http
www.uchile.cl/cultura/lenguas/yaganes/fonologica.doc)
Arnetti, Maria Cristina. 1981. La concezione
mitico-cosmogonica presso gli Yamana. Studi di Letteratura Ispano-Americana 11:
107-123.
Beauvoir, Jose M. 1901 (?). Pequeño diccionario del
idioma fueguino-ona con su correspondiente castellano. Buenos Aires.
———. 1915. Los Shelknam: Indígenas
de la Tierra del Fuego. Buenos Aires.
Borrero, Luis Alberto. 1994. The extermination of the
Selk’nam. In: Burch, Ernest S. and Linda J. Ellanna, (eds.),
Hunter-Gatherer Research, 247-61. Oxford: Berg.
———. 1983. On cultural adaptation in the
subantarctic zone. Current Anthropology 24/2:21–42. doi:10.1086/202981
Bridges, Thomas. 1893. A few notes on the structure of
Yaghan. The Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and
Ireland, XXIII/1:53–81. doi:10.2307/2842315
———. 1933. Yamana–English
Dictionary. Mödling: Anthropos-Institut.
Campbell, Lyle. 1997. American Indian languages. The
historical linguistics of Native American. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Clairis, Christos. 1985. El qawasqar. Lingüístic
Fueguina. Teoría y descripción. Valdivia. Universidad Astral de
Chile. Estudios Filológicos. Anejo 12.
Furlong, Charles Wellington. 1917.The Haush and the Onas,
primitive tribes of Tierra del Fuego. Proceedings of the Nineteenth
International Congress of Americanists, Washington, December 1915. Washington,
D. C.:432–444.
Grimes, Barbara F., ed. 2000. Ethnologue. 14th
Edition. Dallas, TX: SIL International.
Grinevald, Colette. 1998. Language endangerment in South
America: A programmatic approach. In Lenore A. Grenoble and Lindsay J. Whaley,
(eds.), Endangered Languages. Current Issues and Future Prospects, 124–59.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Guerra, Ana María. 1989. Esbozo fonológico del
yagán. Actas del Octavo Seminario de Investigacion y Enseñanza de
la Lingüística. Santiago: Sociedad Chilena de
Lingüística y Universided de Santiago de Chile, V.88–93.
———. 1992. Las fluctuaciones de fonemas en
el yagán. Revista de Lingüística Teórica y Aplicada
30.171–182.
Gusinde, P. Martin. 1926. Das Lautsystem der
feuerländishen Sprachen. Anthropos 21: 1000–1024.
———. 1946. Urmenschen in Feuerland.
Berlin: Paul Zsolnay Verlag.
Guyot, Mireille. 1968. Les mythes chez les Selk’nam et
les Yamana de la Terre de Feu. Paris: Institut de l’Ethnologie.
Hestermann-Hamburg, Ferd. 1929. Das Pronomen im Yamana,
Feuerland. International Journal of American Linguistics
5/2-4.150–179. doi:10.1086/463778
Key, Mary Ritchie. 1981. North and South American Linguistic
Connections. La Linguistique 17/1:3-18.
Key, Mary Ritchie. 1978. Araucanian Genetic Relationships.
International Journal of American Linguistics 44/4: 280-293. doi:10.1086/465556
Klafkowski, Piotr. 1998. Who Remembers the People of
Fireland? Reflections Inspired by Martin Gusinde's Manuscript Remains. A
Contribution to the Study of Linguistic Extermination. Lingua Posnaniensis
40.189-214.
Koppers, Wilhelm. 1927. Die fünf Dialekte in der
Sprache der Yamana auf Feuerland. Anthropos 22.466–476.
Lothrop, Samuel Kirkland. 1928. The Indians of Tierra del
Fuego. New York: Museum of the American Indian, Heye Foundation.
Poblete Mendoza, Maria Teresa and Adalberto Salas. 1999.
Fonemas yámana (yagán). Estructura fonológica de la
palabra. Revista de Lingüistica Teórica y Aplicada
37.107–22.
Salas, Alberto and Maria Teresa Poblete Mendoza. 1997. Pares
minimos y estatus fonemico. Causa o consecuencia? Revista de Lingüistica
Teórica y Aplicada 35.125–33.
Salas, Alberto and Alba Valencia. 1990. El fonetismo del
yámana o yagán. Una nota en lingüistica de salvataje. Revista
de Lingüistica Teórica y Aplicada 28.147–69.
Tovar, Antonio. 1961. Catálogo de las lenguas de
América del Sur. Buenos Aires: Editorial Sudamérica.
Wilbert, Johannes. 1975. Folk literature of the Selknam
Indians. Martin Gusinde’s collection of Selknam narratives. Los Angeles:
UCLA/Latin American Center Publications.
Lenore A. Grenoble
lenore.grenoble@dartmouth.edu
Lindsay J. Whaley
lindsay.whaley@dartmouth.edu
Program in Linguistics & Cognitive Science
6086 Reed Hall
Dartmouth College
Hanover, New Hampshire 03755
[1]The long-term
impact of this kind of thinking on both language vitality and the field of
linguistics has been summarized by Colette Grinevald (1998), pointing to the
urgent need to train and support linguists to undertake serious fieldwork.
[2] See Wilbert
(1975) for Gusinde’s contributions to the collection and study of Ona
literature.
[3] The most up to
date information on speaker populations is available in Grimes (2000) and is as
follows, including most recent date for this information: Tehuelche: 30 speakers
in 1983; Qawasqar: 20 speakers in 1996; Puelche: 5-6 speakers; Yaman: 3 speakers
in 1990; Ona: 1-3 speakers in 1991.
[4] Ona, a Chon
language, along with Tehuelche, was once the majority (principal) language in
Patagonia. Compared to Yaghan, it has been considerably less studied. The
earliest known word list comes from Magellan's chronicler Pigafetta. A
dictionary was published by Beauvoir in 1901, and a longer work with more
phonetic information was published by Beauvoir in (1915). Gusinde (1926)
provides a brief overview of the phonetics of Ona. Furlong collected a small
word list of just under one hundred items (published in Furlong 1917). This
list includes a couple of place names, phrases (such as k’ish pay
‘where is’ or k’ish chain ‘where
walks’) and directions (e.g. kayuke ‘west’;
ke’nen’ek ‘from the west’; and ‘wind from
the west’). Unfortunately, there is no sound recording of the word list to
the best of our knowledge.
[5] The Yaghan here
is presented in a simplified version of Gusinde’s transcription; he
followed the Ellis system of transcription, which uses a number of diacritics
omitted here for readibilty on HTML. Anyone wishing to pursue the issue of
voiced consonants in Yaghan should consult Gusinde’s original article. |